The Feral Child Tied To A Toilet For 13 Years

Vsauce2
14 Jun 202428:57

Summary

TLDRThe script recounts the tragic story of 'Genie,' a 13-year-old girl discovered in 1970, who had been isolated and abused her entire life, providing a unique opportunity for scientists to study the development of human language. Despite initial progress, her linguistic development plateaued, and the case was marred by legal battles and personal conflicts among her caretakers. The narrative explores the ethical dilemmas of scientific research on vulnerable individuals and the quest for knowledge at the expense of human suffering.

Takeaways

  • šŸ˜¢ Genie, a 13-year-old girl, was discovered having spent her entire life in isolation, subjected to severe physical, mental, and emotional abuse.
  • šŸ§¬ Her case was seen as a unique opportunity to study the development of human language, as her brain was considered a 'blank slate' at a critical period for language acquisition.
  • šŸ  Genie's father was hyper-sensitive to sound and had extreme reactions to control noise, leading to Genie's confinement and abuse.
  • šŸŒ The scientific community was divided on whether Genie'såœę»žēš„čÆ­č؀ development supported Chomsky's theory of innate language structure or Lenneberg's critical period hypothesis.
  • šŸ¤ Genie showed significant progress in language and cognitive development under the care of researchers and foster families, but her progress was inconsistent and eventually stalled.
  • šŸ’” Conflicts among researchers and care providers, including disputes over custody and research methodologies, complicated Genie's care and recovery.
  • šŸ“‰ The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) withdrew funding for Genie's research due to concerns about the direction and impact of the studies.
  • šŸ“š The case of Genie raised ethical questions about the treatment of research subjects, especially those who are vulnerable, and the balance between scientific inquiry and human welfare.
  • šŸ” Genie's life after the research project was marked by a series of failed foster care placements, returning her to a state of severe abuse and regression in her development.
  • šŸ” The true identity of 'Genie' was kept anonymous to protect her privacy, highlighting the importance of safeguarding individuals involved in scientific studies.

Q & A

  • What was the significance of Genie's case in the context of scientific research?

    -Genie's case was significant because it presented a unique opportunity to study the effects of extreme isolation and abuse on human development, particularly in relation to language acquisition and neurological development.

  • How did Genie's father's reaction to sound impact the family's lifestyle?

    -Genie's father, Clark, was incensed by any and all sound, leading the family to have no TV or radio. This extreme sensitivity to sound contributed to the creation of an isolated and silent environment within the home.

  • What was the condition of Genie when she was discovered at 13 years old?

    -Genie was discovered at 13 years old weighing less than 60 pounds, walking like an injured bunny, and still wearing a diaper. She understood fewer than 20 words and could not speak beyond uttering a handful of protective phrases.

  • What was the role of Carl Linnaeus in the understanding of Genie's case?

    -Carl Linnaeus, known for formalizing binomial nomenclature and being the father of biological taxonomy, would have classified Genie as 'homo ferus,' suggesting she represented a distinct strain of feral human due to her extreme isolation and lack of socialization.

  • What were the conditions Genie was subjected to during her confinement?

    -Genie was strapped to a potty training chair in a 10ft x 10ft room during the day and at night was put in a crib with wire screens, essentially a cage. She was physically abused and had no exposure to language or social interaction.

  • How did Genie's case coincide with advances in linguistic theory?

    -Genie's case coincided with the publication of Noam Chomsky's 'Syntactic Structures' and Eric Lenneberg's 'Biological Foundations of Language,' which were challenging traditional views on language acquisition and proposing the existence of a critical period for language learning.

  • What was the critical period hypothesis proposed by Eric Lenneberg, and how did it relate to Genie?

    -The critical period hypothesis suggested that humans have a limited timeframe, from 2 years to the onset of puberty, in which they can acquire language naturally. Genie, being discovered at the age of 13, presented a test case to see if language could be learned outside this period.

  • What were the ethical concerns raised by Genie's case in the scientific community?

    -The ethical concerns included the manner in which Genie was studied, the potential exploitation of her tragic circumstances for scientific advancement, and the lack of clear guidelines on how to ethically and effectively rehabilitate and study a person in her unique situation.

  • How did Genie's linguistic progress initially and what challenges did she face later?

    -Initially, Genie showed remarkable progress, moving from a vocabulary of about 20 words to over 100 and starting to form simple sentences. However, her progress plateaued, particularly in her Broca's area development, which is responsible for grammar, raising questions about the limits of language acquisition in her case.

  • What were the conflicts that arose during Genie's case and what were their impacts?

    -Conflicts arose between researchers over the direction and focus of the study, between the research project and the National Institute of Mental Health over funding, and within Genie's own life as she was moved between different care situations. These conflicts led to a lack of consistent care, disrupted research, and ultimately, a negative impact on Genie's well-being and rehabilitation.

  • What was the ultimate fate of Genie, and what does her story say about the intersection of science, ethics, and human suffering?

    -Genie's story ended with her returning to state care, and the last known information suggests she was alive but in a state of severe regression, refusing to speak. Her story highlights the complex ethical dilemmas in scientific research, especially when it involves vulnerable individuals, and the potential for good intentions to lead to tragic outcomes.

Outlines

00:00

šŸ˜¢ The Tragic Discovery of Genie

The paragraph introduces the harrowing case of Genie, a 13-year-old girl who had been locked away her entire life, suffering from extreme physical, mental, and emotional abuse. This isolation made her a unique subject for studying human language development due to her brain being a 'blank slate.' The narrative delves into the dark circumstances of her life, including the abuse by her father and the tragic death of her sibling. Genie's limited vocabulary and inability to perform basic human functions like chewing solid food highlight the severity of her condition. The paragraph sets the stage for a discussion on the ethical dilemmas of scientific research and the potential insights Genie's case could offer into human language acquisition.

05:05

šŸŒ± The Feral Child and the Mystery of Language Development

This paragraph explores the rarity of cases like Genie's, often referred to as 'feral children,' and the challenges in studying them due to high mortality rates. It raises critical questions about the potential for language learning in individuals who have had no exposure to language during their formative years. The discussion then pivots to the complexities of understanding language, using the analogy of bread to illustrate the nuances and variations in language that even a typical person must learn. The paragraph also touches on the revolutionary linguistic theories of Noam Chomsky, which suggest an innate human capacity for language structure, challenging traditional views of language learning. It sets the stage for examining Genie's case in the context of these theories.

10:06

šŸ”¬ The Scientific Scrutiny of an Isolated Mind

The focus of this paragraph is on the scientific community's reaction to Genie's case and the research that ensued. It discusses the critical period hypothesis proposed by Eric Lenneberg, which posits a limited window for natural language acquisition. Genie, being at the end of this period, became a subject of immense interest. The narrative describes the scramble among researchers to study her, reflecting the ethical and practical dilemmas of such an unprecedented case. The paragraph also touches on the initial medical assessments and the broad scope of the research, which aimed to understand the potential of Genie's brain after years of sensory deprivation.

15:07

šŸ¤” The Cognitive Awakening of Genie

This paragraph delves into Genie's initial interactions with the world outside her isolation, highlighting her struggle to adapt and the challenges she faced in perceiving her environment. It discusses the gradual development of her cognitive abilities, particularly her emerging language skills, as she began to form basic words and understand concepts like 'doctor' and 'tie.' The narrative captures the excitement and optimism of the researchers as they witnessed Genie's progress, which included the development of object permanence and the ability to express simple desires. The paragraph also reflects on the emotional bond that formed between Genie and her caregivers, indicating her potential for further development.

20:08

šŸ“‰ The Stalled Progress and Controversies

The paragraph discusses the stagnation in Genie's linguistic progress and the ensuing controversies. It outlines the conflicts between researchers, the National Institute of Mental Health's withdrawal of funding due to unfocused research, and the internal struggles within Genie's brain development. The narrative also touches on the personal dynamics and power struggles among the researchers, leading to a breakdown in the research collaboration. Furthermore, it describes the legal battles and the tragic series of abusive foster care placements that Genie endured after the research project concluded, ultimately leading to a regression in her communication abilities and a return to silence.

25:11

šŸš The Aftermath and Legacy of Genie's Story

This final paragraph reflects on the tragic aftermath of Genie's story, detailing her return to state care and the subsequent lawsuits and disputes over her well-being. It highlights the contrasting actions of those involved, from Susan Curtiss's genuine concern and donations to Genie's estate to the self-serving behaviors of others. The paragraph concludes with a poignant description of Genie's 27th birthday and the realization that her true identity and wishes were overshadowed by the ambitions of those around her. The narrative ends on a somber note, leaving the audience to contemplate the ethical implications of Genie's case and the human cost of scientific curiosity.

Mindmap

Keywords

šŸ’”Feral Child

A 'feral child' refers to a child who has lived isolated from human contact, often raised by animals or in extreme cases of neglect. In the video, Genie is described as a 'feral child' due to her extreme isolation and lack of human interaction, which makes her case a subject of study for understanding human development and language acquisition.

šŸ’”Linguistic Theory

Linguistic theory encompasses the scientific study of the nature, structure, and variation of language. The video discusses how Genie's case coincided with advances in linguistic theory, particularly Chomsky's theory of 'universal grammar' and Lenneberg's 'critical period hypothesis,' which suggest that there is a specific window in childhood during which language is most easily acquired.

šŸ’”Isolation

Isolation, in the context of the video, refers to Genie's lack of social interaction and sensory stimulation, which is a critical factor in her developmental delays and language acquisition challenges. The video explores the devastating effects of such isolation on Genie's cognitive and emotional development.

šŸ’”Child Abuse

Child abuse is a general term for any harmful act or behavior towards children. In the video, Genie's case is described as one of the most tragic cases of child abuse, involving physical, mental, and emotional torment, which raises questions about the morality of the scientific study conducted on her.

šŸ’”Critical Period Hypothesis

The critical period hypothesis, as mentioned in the video, is the idea that there is a specific period in early life during which language acquisition is most effective. Genie's case was of interest because she was discovered outside of this period, leading researchers to question whether language could still be learned after this 'critical period' had passed.

šŸ’”Object Permanence

Object permanence is the understanding that objects continue to exist even when not within one's line of sight. The video describes Genie's development of object permanence as a significant milestone, indicating her growing cognitive abilities and understanding of the world beyond her immediate perception.

šŸ’”Egocentrism

Egocentrism is the inability to differentiate between one's own perspective and that of others. The video describes Genie's initial state as one of 'clinical egocentrism,' where her world was entirely centered on herself, reflecting the lack of social interaction and understanding of others' existence.

šŸ’”Language Acquisition

Language acquisition refers to the process of learning a language. The video discusses Genie's progress in language acquisition as a key area of study, highlighting her initial limitations and the challenges she faced in developing language skills due to her isolated upbringing.

šŸ’”Mental Retardation

Mental retardation, now more commonly referred to as intellectual disability, is a reduced ability to understand new or complex information. The video mentions that Genie was tested for mental retardation, but her results were inconclusive, indicating the complexity of her condition and the impact of her isolation on her cognitive development.

šŸ’”Ethical Considerations

Ethical considerations in research involve the moral principles that guide the treatment of human subjects. The video raises ethical questions about the study of Genie, as her case was exploited for scientific progress without adequate consideration for her well-being and consent.

šŸ’”Scientific Tragedy

A scientific tragedy refers to a situation where the pursuit of scientific knowledge leads to unintended negative consequences. The video's title and narrative frame Genie's story as a scientific tragedy, highlighting the harm caused by the research and the failure to protect her interests.

Highlights

The case of a 13-year-old girl, Genie, who had been locked in a room her entire life, presents a tragic yet scientifically intriguing study on the development of human language.

Genie's limited exposure to language and social interaction made her a unique subject for understanding the critical period for language acquisition.

Genie's father's extreme aversion to sound and the family's isolation contributed to her abnormal development.

The discovery of Genie at 13 years old, with the mental and physical abilities of a toddler, shocked the scientific community.

Genie's case was seen as a potential Rosetta Stone for understanding human language development due to her brain's 'blank slate'.

The video explores the ethical dilemmas of using Genie's tragic life for scientific advancement.

Genie's initial vocabulary was extremely limited, with fewer than 20 words, highlighting the severity of her isolation.

The absence of language input during Genie's formative years raises questions about the innate vs. learned aspects of language.

Genie's case coincided with Noam Chomsky's revolutionary linguistic theories, providing a real-world test for his ideas.

The video discusses the critical period hypothesis and its implications for Genie's potential to learn language.

Genie's progress in language acquisition was initially rapid, suggesting a natural capacity for language despite her late start.

The video highlights the complex ethical and legal battles that arose from Genie's case, including disputes over her care and research access.

Genie's story illustrates the devastating long-term effects of child abuse and neglect on cognitive and emotional development.

The scientific community's handling of Genie's case raises questions about the balance between research and the welfare of human subjects.

Genie's eventual withdrawal from speaking and her return to a state of silence underscore the profound impact of her abuse.

The video concludes with a reflection on Genie's life, the scientific knowledge gained, and the ethical costs of such research.

Transcripts

play00:04

One of the most horrific cases ofĀ  childhood tragedy ever documentedĀ Ā 

play00:08

will make you question the moralityĀ  of modern science -- and bread.

play00:14

A 13 year old girl who had spent her entireĀ  life locked in a room, physically, mentally,Ā Ā 

play00:20

and emotionally abused, was somehow a mix ofĀ  petri dish and Rosetta Stone that could unlockĀ Ā 

play00:27

the mysteries of human language -- because herĀ  brain was a blank slate at exactly the right time.

play00:34

Itā€™s a story of good intentions, horribleĀ  execution, greed, scholarly ambition,Ā Ā 

play00:40

and the deepest depths of human suffering --Ā  all in the service of scientific progress.

play00:47

When you spend the most importantĀ  developmental years of your life in totalĀ Ā 

play00:53

isolation, a decade of deranged solitaryĀ  confinement and torture -- what happens?

play01:04

Genieā€™s discovery in 1970 was an accident. HerĀ  mother, Irene Wiley, was nearly blind from aĀ Ā 

play01:11

mix of cataracts and neurological damage fromĀ  being hit by the handcrank of a wringer washer,Ā Ā 

play01:17

and thatā€™s why she walked into the wrong socialĀ  services office with her daughter -- a 13 year oldĀ Ā 

play01:24

who weighed less than 60 pounds, who walked likeĀ  an injured bunny, and who still wore a diaper.Ā Ā 

play01:31

She understood fewer than 20 words, like ā€œMotherā€Ā  and the colors red and blue. She spat and clawed,Ā Ā 

play01:39

and she couldnā€™t speak beyond uttering aĀ  handful of protective phrases like ā€œstopit.ā€

play01:45

Carl Linnaeus, the formalizer of binomialĀ  nomenclature and the father of biologicalĀ Ā 

play01:50

taxonomy, would have classified Genie asĀ  homo ferus, a distinctly separate strainĀ Ā 

play01:56

of feral human. A New York Times writerĀ  classified her as a ā€œshattered creature.ā€

play02:03

Genieā€™s father Clark was incensed by anyĀ  and all sound. The family had no TV orĀ Ā 

play02:10

radio. Clark and Ireneā€™s first childĀ  died after being placed in the garage,Ā Ā 

play02:16

probably to create distance from the crying.Ā  The 2 month old infant succumbed to what wasĀ Ā 

play02:21

deemed to be ā€œquick pneumonia,ā€ butĀ  which was almost certainly exposure.

play02:26

Genie was tortured much more slowly.

play02:31

During the day she was strapped to a pottyĀ  training chair in a 10ft x 10ft room. ThereĀ Ā 

play02:36

were two shaded windows and a single lightĀ  bulb and nothing on the salmon-colored walls.Ā Ā 

play02:42

A piece of wood leaned against one corner --Ā  Clark would beat Genie with it to punish herĀ Ā 

play02:47

for making noise. At night she was put inĀ  a crib with wire screens, basically a cage,Ā Ā 

play02:53

and she was wrapped in a sleeping bag thatĀ  researchers described as a straitjacket.Ā Ā 

play02:59

She occasionally was allowed to play with twoĀ  vinyl raincoats and an empty cottage cheeseĀ Ā 

play03:06

container -- and when social workers tried to feedĀ  her, they discovered that she couldnā€™t chew food.

play03:14

After a lifelong diet of baby food and otherĀ  soft foods, she simply didnā€™t know how.

play03:20

Genieā€™s life was the most tragicĀ  case of child abuse anyone involvedĀ Ā 

play03:25

in her care had ever seen -- or would ever see.

play03:29

And Genie also presentedĀ  one of the most incredibleĀ Ā 

play03:33

scientific opportunities in modern history:

play03:36

ā€œThe girl was apparently uttering infantile noisesĀ  and still wearing diapers discovered the case twoĀ Ā 

play03:42

weeks ago, but the authorities are hoping sheĀ  still may have a normal learning capacity.ā€

play03:47

That was the question: what could a humanĀ  being subjected to a lifetime of sensory,Ā Ā 

play03:54

experiential, and social deprivation actuallyĀ  learn once immersed inā€¦ normal life? And theĀ Ā 

play04:01

most interesting course of study was aroundĀ  language -- because Genieā€™s life coincidedĀ Ā 

play04:07

perfectly with advances in linguistic theory thatĀ  challenged literally thousands of years of wisdom.

play04:14

Real quick, this video is sponsored by BetterHelp.

play04:17

My video about happiness explored the complexityĀ  of how we conceptualize happiness as a state ofĀ Ā 

play04:23

mind vs its impacts as an emotion. And howĀ  the best way to stumble upon happiness isĀ Ā 

play04:29

through pursuing general human flourishing. ItĀ  could be useful to talk through your challengesĀ Ā 

play04:36

with one of BetterHelpā€™s over 30,000 licensedĀ  therapists. And use my link BetterHelp.com/Vsauce.

play04:43

Just fill out a questionnaire to helpĀ  assess your specific needs and getĀ Ā 

play04:46

matched with a therapist often in less thanĀ  48 hours. You can choose to do a phone call,Ā Ā 

play04:51

video call or messaging on aĀ  schedule that works for you.

play04:55

Over 4 million people have usedĀ  BetterHelp to find the right therapistĀ Ā 

play05:00

who will listen and provide unbiased advice.Ā  And if the therapist youā€™re first matched withĀ Ā 

play05:05

doesnā€™t seem like a fit you can switchĀ  easily to a new one at no extra cost.

play05:10

So click the link below to help support Vsauce2,Ā  and get 10% off your first month of BetterHelp soĀ Ā 

play05:15

you can connect with a therapist and see ifĀ  it helps you. Thatā€™s BetterHelp.com/Vsauce.

play05:21

There had been a few notable cases of the ā€˜feralĀ  childā€™ who existed in as close to a HobbesianĀ Ā 

play05:27

state of nature as weā€™re likely to get -- startingĀ  with Jean Itardā€™s study of Victor of Aveyron,Ā Ā 

play05:34

a ā€œwild childā€ who emerged from a forest inĀ  France in 1800. Why donā€™t we have thousandsĀ Ā 

play05:41

of cases like Victor and Genie to examine?Ā  Wellā€¦ most people donā€™t abandon kids inĀ Ā 

play05:47

the woods or tie them to a potty trainingĀ  chair all day -- and whether itā€™s isolation,Ā Ā 

play05:52

neglect, or abuseā€¦ kids in their circumstancesĀ  tend to die before they can be rehabilitatedĀ Ā 

play05:59

physically and mentally, let alone beĀ  studied fully and ethically by scientists.

play06:06

But Genie checked all the boxes the naturalĀ  world never really lets us check. Could aĀ Ā 

play06:12

girl who had virtually no exposure toĀ  language learn how to communicate withĀ Ā 

play06:16

words despite being on the precipice ofĀ  adulthood? Can a teenager learn to talk?

play06:23

WHAT IS BREAD?

play06:25

Youā€™ve spent a lifetime learningĀ  about the nuances of bread withoutĀ Ā 

play06:29

even realizing it -- and Genie was startingĀ  with literally nothing at 13 or 14 years old.

play06:36

First, itā€™s got a name. Bread, and eventuallyĀ  you learn that name. But is bread only the wholeĀ Ā 

play06:42

thing? When it gets sliced or ripped up, are thoseĀ  pieces also bread? Is there a new name for that,Ā Ā 

play06:50

or does it have to be qualified with aĀ  new word like ā€œslice?ā€ ā€¦ which doesnā€™tĀ Ā 

play06:55

sound anything like bread. How smallĀ  can slices get before it stops beingĀ Ā 

play07:00

bread and suddenly turns into a crumb?Ā  If thereā€™s a new name when it gets tiny,Ā Ā 

play07:05

why isnā€™t there a new name when itā€™s a hugeĀ  collection, like on a rack at a bakery?

play07:10

This is French Peasant bread. Are the brownĀ  ones like pumpernickel also bread even thoughĀ Ā 

play07:16

it looks nothing like this and tastes completelyĀ  different? Fluffy, leavened bread and compressedĀ Ā 

play07:22

unleavened flatbreads are both bread. But ifĀ  itā€™s called ā€œdoughā€ and not bread when itā€™sĀ Ā 

play07:27

squishy before itā€™s baked, is it still calledĀ  bread when itā€™s molded on the counter for aĀ Ā 

play07:33

month? Is the hard outside a distinctly separateĀ  entity from the soft inside, with different names,Ā Ā 

play07:39

or is it all just bread? If the individualĀ  pieces like the crust have different names,Ā Ā 

play07:45

why donā€™t we list all those pieces when we talkĀ  about it? Why do we just lump together all thoseĀ Ā 

play07:50

separate parts as ā€œbread,ā€ and how do we knowĀ  when the components stop and the whole begins?

play07:56

You eventually discover that bread is foodĀ  -- is it bread both before youā€™ve eaten itĀ Ā 

play08:02

and as you completely transform it in yourĀ  mouth, and then pass it through your body?Ā Ā 

play08:07

Is it still bread as you chew on it, butĀ  stops being bread when you swallow? Why?!

play08:12

And if we make a sandwich out of it, why do weĀ  just stop noticing the bread? Itā€™s still there,Ā Ā 

play08:17

itā€™s the top and the bottom of the thing,Ā Ā 

play08:18

but all of a sudden weā€™re talking aboutĀ  a totally new word. SANDWICH. Thatā€¦ hasĀ Ā 

play08:22

nothing to do with the sand at the beachĀ  or Halloween decorations, orā€¦ bread.

play08:27

If I have two of the same loaf, whyĀ  would I talk about the bread as aĀ Ā 

play08:31

singular unit -- but if I sampledĀ  a few different breads I wouldnā€™t?

play08:37

And WHAT IS TOAST?

play08:39

These questions constitute only aĀ  portion of understanding what oneĀ Ā 

play08:44

single word in one language actually means --Ā  and vocabulary isnā€™t even the interesting part.

play08:51

Two things were born in 1957: one wasĀ  Genie. The other was a linguistic upheavalĀ Ā 

play08:58

so significant that its publication isĀ  often just referred to as ā€œThe Event.ā€

play09:04

Noam Chomsky was almost completelyĀ  unknown when he published ā€œSyntacticĀ Ā 

play09:08

Structures,ā€ which argued that humansĀ  donā€™t learn or acquire grammar andĀ Ā 

play09:14

syntax -- theyā€™re born with a built-inĀ  sense of language construction that isĀ Ā 

play09:19

wholly separate from the development ofĀ  semantics, or the meaning of language.

play09:24

Around 300 BC, Epicurus theorized thatĀ  man developed sounds as a reaction toĀ Ā 

play09:29

what he saw and experienced in nature,Ā  but that only explains how we got words.Ā Ā 

play09:34

Most everyone before and since thoughtĀ  language was a mixture of gift and curseĀ Ā 

play09:40

from deities -- itā€™s such a complex phenomenonĀ  that there couldnā€™t be any other explanation.

play09:46

Chomskyā€™s theory rejected all of that -- includingĀ Ā 

play09:49

the notion that we learn languageĀ  from the other people around us,Ā Ā 

play09:53

especially our parents. They just built onĀ  a universal grammar we already contained.

play10:00

By the time Genie materialized in 1970, theĀ  field of linguistics had spent more than aĀ Ā 

play10:05

decade at war with itself over the validityĀ  of Chomskyā€™s ā€œSyntactic Structuresā€ -- andĀ Ā 

play10:11

Eric Lennebergā€™s ā€œBiological Foundations ofĀ  Languageā€ complicated things even further byĀ Ā 

play10:17

suggesting the critical period hypothesis --Ā  that humans have a limited timeframe from 2Ā Ā 

play10:23

years to the onset of puberty in whichĀ  they can acquire language naturally.

play10:29

Genie wasā€¦ a perfectly imperfect testĀ  subject. She was as close to a blankĀ Ā 

play10:35

slate as any researcher would ever get. HerĀ  ā€˜critical periodā€™ window had just closed,Ā Ā 

play10:42

and they were about to find out whether sheĀ  had a natural grammar and syntax programmedĀ Ā 

play10:47

before birth. Was Chomsky right, or was itĀ  Lenneberg? Were they both right? Or neither?!

play10:55

Genie spent the first 13 yearsĀ  of her life in total isolation.Ā Ā 

play10:59

She spent the next 13 years under the spotlightĀ  in the center ring of a big-top science circus.

play11:08

To be fair, no one really knew what to do withĀ  her. There was no playbook for this kind of case,Ā Ā 

play11:15

and there were no rules beyond the law and generalĀ  morality. Genie had been taken immediately out ofĀ Ā 

play11:21

the home -- on the morning Clark and his wife wereĀ  to appear in court on charges of willful abuse ofĀ Ā 

play11:27

a minor, he shot himself with a revolver. He leftĀ  2 notes -- one to his son John, Genieā€™s brother,Ā Ā 

play11:36

whose testicles Clark had routinelyĀ  assaulted with a 3-foot board: John said,Ā Ā 

play11:43

ā€œI donā€™t think he wanted me to have children, andĀ  itā€™s a wonder I did,ā€ and that, "He would write meĀ Ā 

play11:50

a note excusing me from gym so the kids didn't seeĀ  my privates in the showers." The note to John toldĀ Ā 

play11:56

him where clothes would be for the funeral, andĀ  it ended with, ā€œI love you. Goodbye and be good.ā€

play12:04

The other note was for the police. ItĀ  said, ā€œThe world will never understand.ā€

play12:11

The Childrenā€™s Hospitalā€™s immediate concernĀ  was Genieā€™s physical health and safety,Ā Ā 

play12:16

and professionals from several disciplinesĀ  tried to assess the situation. Dr. JamesĀ Ā 

play12:22

Kent was the first to examine her -- he said,Ā  ā€œAs far as Iā€™m concerned, Genie was the mostĀ Ā 

play12:27

profoundly damaged child Iā€™ve ever seen,ā€ and heĀ  concluded that, ā€œGenieā€™s life was a wasteland.ā€

play12:35

Jay Shurleyā€™s field of expertise was the brainĀ  -- he conducted sleep analysis that showedĀ Ā 

play12:40

a high number of neural oscillations calledĀ  ā€œsleep spindles,ā€ indicating to him that GenieĀ Ā 

play12:46

had a degree of mental retardation. But despiteĀ  having very low functionality on some measures,Ā Ā 

play12:53

she was perfectly normal on other tests thatĀ  would have indicated a mental disability.

play12:58

What Genie did have was an incalculable numberĀ  of problems and mysteries. Shurley said,Ā Ā 

play13:05

ā€œFrom being a totally neglected waif at the time IĀ  did my consultation, by the time I came back GenieĀ Ā 

play13:11

had become a prize. There was a contest aboutĀ  who was going to investigate her, and how.ā€

play13:18

Dr. David Rigler led the application for grantĀ  funding to study Genie through the NationalĀ Ā 

play13:23

Institute of Mental Health -- in February of 1971,Ā  they were approved for $21,500, the equivalent ofĀ Ā 

play13:31

about $150,000 today, to run through September ofĀ  that year. What was actually going to be studied?

play13:39

Everything.

play13:40

What could a brain dormant for 14 years,Ā  undernourished and interspersed with boutsĀ Ā 

play13:45

of what amounted to torture, eventually learnĀ  to do? Was it possible for researchers toĀ Ā 

play13:51

even measure Genieā€™s progress, when their humanĀ  subject first had to get used toā€¦ other humans?

play13:57

And the first real experimentĀ  here was Genie studying Genie.

play14:03

Normal human development consists of seeing,Ā  hearing, touching, and experiencing things,Ā Ā 

play14:08

all guided by veterans of humanity who haveĀ  an interest in facilitating your advancement:Ā Ā 

play14:14

thatā€™s usually your parents and family. YouĀ  eventually learn that one person whoā€™s aroundĀ Ā 

play14:19

all the time is some version of ā€œmama,ā€ whoĀ  is a distinctly separate being from yourself,Ā Ā 

play14:25

and the big furry loud thing is a ā€œdog,ā€ andĀ  you like ā€œeating.ā€ But you donā€™t like eatingĀ Ā 

play14:31

ā€œbroccoli,ā€ especially when youā€™d rather beĀ  watching ā€œCocomelon.ā€ And you begin to developĀ Ā 

play14:36

an internal monologue using those terms thatĀ  draws on this increasingly-organized catalogĀ Ā 

play14:42

of information with greater efficiencyĀ  and precision. The more you do and see,Ā Ā 

play14:48

the larger that database, and the more youĀ  make use of it, the better you get at it.

play14:55

How does a child think, ā€œI want to watchĀ  Cocomelonā€ when they have no sense of theĀ Ā 

play15:00

act of sitting down and watching a TV show, orĀ  who Cocomelon is, or why he doesnā€™t look likeĀ Ā 

play15:06

a real life person, or why itā€™s nicerĀ  to watch when the dog isnā€™t barking,Ā Ā 

play15:11

or why ā€œmamaā€ trying to force ā€œbroccoliā€ down yourĀ  gullet is a distraction -- when that child doesnā€™tĀ Ā 

play15:17

know what anything is, what anything is called,Ā  or how to express their relationship to any of it?

play15:26

What can even go on in their head?

play15:29

That was Genie, but with the entire worldĀ  -- and she was starting at 14. For her,Ā Ā 

play15:37

even seeing things was hard. She had troubleĀ  focusing on anything beyond 10 feet away,Ā Ā 

play15:43

because sheā€™d spent her life in aĀ  10 x 10 room -- she literally hadĀ Ā 

play15:48

never had to process with her eyesĀ  anything further away than the wall.

play15:54

She slowly began to get comfortable with theĀ  medical doctors and researchers -- which wasĀ Ā 

play15:59

a serious conceptual hurdle. GenieĀ  had existed in a state of clinicalĀ Ā 

play16:04

egocentrism -- her entire worldĀ  was just her. Now it was her andĀ Ā 

play16:10

other people -- but Dr. Kent felt thatĀ  she could improve with relationships:

play16:16

ā€œUp until one particular day, GenieĀ  didnā€™t seem to respond in any specialĀ Ā 

play16:22

way to my coming or going at the end ofĀ  our sessions. Then one day when I leftĀ Ā 

play16:27

her expression changed from happy to sad, toĀ  indicate that there was some sadness in theĀ Ā 

play16:35

separation for her. And that was the firstĀ  indication I had that we were beginning toĀ Ā 

play16:40

form this relationship. I thought if as longĀ  as she had the capacity to form attachments,Ā Ā 

play16:45

she had the capacity to learn,Ā  and the capacity to get better.ā€

play16:47

And then something incredible happened.

play16:52

Thereā€™s a video of a session with Genie thatĀ  shows Jean Butler -- a special educationĀ Ā 

play16:56

teacher who would become a major player inĀ  Genieā€™s tragic life -- trying to get GenieĀ Ā 

play17:01

to tie her own shoes. Genie doesnā€™tĀ  want to do it -- and linguist SusanĀ Ā 

play17:07

Curtiss noted that she never really initiatedĀ  anything, because how would she even know to?

play17:16

BUTLER: ā€œYou do it, and then we canĀ  tell Doctor Kent what you can do.ā€

play17:22

GENIE: ā€œDoctor!ā€

play17:22

BUTLER: ā€œDoctor Kent! RightĀ  there is says that, doesnā€™t it?

play17:25

GENIE: ā€œDoctor!ā€

play17:25

BUTLER: ā€œDoctor Kent. You tie it?ā€

play17:28

GENIE: ā€œTie!ā€

play17:29

BUTLER: ā€œTie it.ā€

play17:29

A lot is happening here. In just a few months,Ā Ā 

play17:32

Genieā€™s vocabulary had grown from aboutĀ  20 words to 100 -- if words were crayons,Ā Ā 

play17:38

thatā€™s like going from a small set of basicĀ  colors like red and blue to a big box thatĀ Ā 

play17:42

even contains ā€˜Chicken Nugget Golden-Brown.ā€™Ā  Itā€™s not 32-bit, but itā€™s serious progress.

play17:49

She knows that ā€œdoctorā€ refers to Doctor Kent --Ā  who is not in the room. It means sheā€™d developedĀ Ā 

play17:56

ā€œobject permanenceā€ -- she knows Doctor KentĀ  exists even when heā€™s not there. Sheā€™s smilingĀ Ā 

play18:03

and happy when she says it -- sheā€™s proud ofĀ  knowing the word, sheā€™s happy about making JeanĀ Ā 

play18:08

Butler happy by saying it, she understands thatĀ  the conceptual Doctor Kent will be happy, too.

play18:16

She was starting to understandĀ  how the world worked.

play18:21

A headline proclaimed ā€œDoctors JudgeĀ  Isolated Girl Able to Learnā€ -- rightĀ Ā 

play18:26

next to ā€œSkinnies May Take it Easy,ā€ aboutĀ  how itā€™s okay for underweight people to relax.

play18:32

And the researchers all adored Genie. She was aĀ  kind of prize and opportunity for them -- but herĀ Ā 

play18:39

innocence and emerging personality was magnetic.Ā  Susan Curtiss was particularly taken by her:

play18:46

CURTISS: ā€œI could tell as all of us couldĀ  by looking at her that there was a lot toĀ Ā 

play18:51

Genie. And that what we had to do was to makeĀ  sure we gave her opportunities to express,Ā Ā 

play18:58

find a way to take what was latentĀ  and express it, or somehow then,Ā Ā 

play19:03

you know, acquire it because theĀ  potential just seemed so great.ā€

play19:07

Genieā€™s progress was remarkable.Ā  And that was part of the problem.

play19:12

DOCTOR: ā€œOh, we were off in a million directions.Ā  Each expert, each person in their own disciplineĀ Ā 

play19:19

thought, oh wow, I can do this, I can do that.Ā  We were flooded with our own associations andĀ Ā 

play19:27

with the pursuits that each of us mightĀ  have thought, wow, look what we can do,Ā Ā 

play19:32

look what we might learn from this, aĀ  girl who has appeared out of nowhere.ā€

play19:38

As the NIMH grant progressed, and was renewed,Ā Ā 

play19:41

they started to ask questions aboutā€¦ what wasĀ  really going on with the research. To them,Ā Ā 

play19:48

it seemed unfocused. It wasnā€™t clear whatĀ  anyone was trying to accomplish scientifically.

play19:54

Susan Curtissā€™s work on Genieā€™s linguisticĀ  development was exceptional -- she tracked howĀ Ā 

play20:01

Genie eventually produced two-word phrases withĀ  verbs, like a subject + verb construction -- ā€œDaveĀ Ā 

play20:08

hurt,ā€ or ā€œCurtiss comeā€ -- and verb + objectĀ  constructions like ā€œshake hand.ā€ Bbecause sheĀ Ā 

play20:15

was thinking in language, not simply mimickingĀ  the adults around her. She occasionally usedĀ Ā 

play20:22

plurals, and her emotional development led toĀ  reactions like, ā€œGo way, Joel, finish story!ā€

play20:29

Perhaps the most important development wasĀ  that Susan Curtiss became Genieā€™s friend.

play20:36

And then three wars broke out: one betweenĀ  David Rigler and Jean Butler, one betweenĀ Ā 

play20:43

the research project and the National InstituteĀ  of Mental Health, and one inside Genieā€™s brain.

play20:50

Because Genieā€™s linguistic progress justā€¦Ā  stopped. The brainā€™s language center isĀ Ā 

play20:57

divided into Wernickeā€™s area and Brocaā€™s area --Ā  Wernickeā€™s area handles speech processing, Brocaā€™sĀ Ā 

play21:03

deals with grammar. Genieā€™s Broca developmentĀ  hit a wall, and it was impossible to tell whetherĀ Ā 

play21:09

it was the limits of her abnormally-developedĀ  brain, or the limits of language acquisition.Ā Ā 

play21:15

Was research on Genie proving that Chomsky orĀ  Lenneberg were right, or that they were wrong?

play21:24

Ehhhh?

play21:26

And that meant the NIMH really had no idea what itĀ  was continuing to fund. They eventually pulled theĀ Ā 

play21:34

grants because David Rigler and most of the otherĀ  researchers were, in the eyes of the Institute,Ā Ā 

play21:40

asking for money to sort ofā€¦ just see whatĀ  happened to Genie. David Rigler tried to explain:

play21:48

DAVID RIGLER: ā€œNobody knew exactly how to dealĀ  with this. We were doing our best -- we got theĀ Ā 

play21:55

best kind of counsel, the best kind of advice,Ā  we had to sort that out and take that as it came,Ā Ā 

play22:01

but we did the very best we knew how, andĀ  we believed that nobody knew any better.ā€

play22:06

Thatā€™sā€¦ that is not a strong grant proposal.

play22:10

But the conflict over how Genie would liveĀ  -- and with whom -- destroyed everything.

play22:18

Genie had been at the Childrenā€™s Hospital,Ā  until Jean Butler -- the special educationĀ Ā 

play22:23

teacher -- said she had been exposed toĀ  German measles, and had exposed Genie asĀ Ā 

play22:27

well. So Genie was quarantined in Butlerā€™sĀ  home, and both of them seemed to like thatĀ Ā 

play22:33

arrangement -- so she stayed. Butler claimedĀ  fantastic progress with language, emotions,Ā Ā 

play22:40

manners, physical health -- though researchersĀ  were never quite able to actually verify it.

play22:46

Butler even kicked theĀ  researchers out for the summer,Ā Ā 

play22:50

claiming it was too much strain onĀ  Genie. Susan Curtiss was concerned:

play22:56

CURTISS: ā€œ... we didnā€™t have such a fun summer.Ā  It was a summer that included weeks of concernĀ Ā 

play23:01

and anxiety and anger and all kinds of thingsĀ  because he and I were kicked out of Jeanā€™s house.ā€

play23:07

INTERVIEWER: ā€œWhy?ā€

play23:08

CURTISS: ā€œWhy? She was crazyā€¦ she didnā€™t wantĀ  the other attachments. I mean, she wanted,Ā Ā 

play23:13

she used to walk around saying ā€˜I amĀ  going to be the next Annie Sullivan,Ā Ā 

play23:18

this girl is going to make me famous.ā€™

play23:20

Annie Sullivan was the nurseĀ  who rescued Helen Keller.

play23:24

Jean Butlerā€™s application to become Genieā€™s fosterĀ  parent was denied -- and then Genie spent 4 yearsĀ Ā 

play23:31

living with David Rigler and his family, includingĀ  his wife who was doing her own work with Genie. ItĀ Ā 

play23:37

is extremely unorthodox for the lead researcher onĀ  a project to have the main research subject liveĀ Ā 

play23:44

with them -- but it did give Genie an opportunityĀ  to function as part of a real family for 4 years.

play23:51

When the research project concluded in 1975, GenieĀ  went back to live with her mother in the home sheĀ Ā 

play23:58

grew up in. Her mother had been considered byĀ  the courts to be blameless in her abuse, andĀ Ā 

play24:04

Genie had a bond with her -- but Genie requiredĀ  24/7 care and her mom just couldnā€™t manage it.

play24:11

So Genie went into a series of foster placements,Ā  and each was more physically and mentally abusiveĀ Ā 

play24:18

than the last. She eventually came back toĀ  the Childrenā€™s Hospital -- where doctorsĀ Ā 

play24:24

found that the foster home had used anĀ  ice cream stick to extract feces fromĀ Ā 

play24:29

her when she had become constipated. And GenieĀ  refused to open her mouth at all. Even eating,Ā Ā 

play24:37

she forced the food between herĀ  lips. It turned out that she hadĀ Ā 

play24:42

been severely beaten for vomiting at theĀ  last foster home -- and in Genieā€™s mind,Ā Ā 

play24:48

if you never open your mouth, you canā€™t vomit,Ā  and if you canā€™t vomit, you canā€™t be beaten.

play24:54

So she went back to silence andĀ  using hand signals to communicate.

play25:00

Genieā€™s mother, backed by Jean Butler, sued theĀ  hospital and its researchers for excessive andĀ Ā 

play25:05

outrageous testing on Genie. David RiglerĀ  submitted a demand for owed compensation toĀ Ā 

play25:11

Genieā€™s miniscule estate -- which was a fewĀ  thousand dollars controlled by John Miner,Ā Ā 

play25:16

a lawyer who had investigated the deathsĀ  of Marilyn Monroe and Robert Kennedy,Ā Ā 

play25:20

and who now had control over Genieā€™s affairsĀ  after becoming enamored with the wholeĀ Ā 

play25:25

case. Heā€™d actually bought the gun Genieā€™sĀ  father had shot himself with as a memento.

play25:32

What began as an incredible opportunity to giveĀ  an abused girl the chance at life she never had,Ā Ā 

play25:40

and to learn about how the human mindĀ  worked, turned into a nightmarishĀ Ā 

play25:47

garbage plate of lawsuits, money,Ā  and self-serving rescue fantasies.

play25:53

Genie went back into state care.Ā  In Genie: A Scientific Tragedy,Ā Ā 

play25:58

Russ Rymer described her 27th birthday party:

play26:01

ā€œA large, bumbling woman with a facialĀ  expression of cowlike incomprehension ā€¦ herĀ Ā 

play26:07

eyes focus poorly on the cake. Her darkĀ  hair has been hacked off raggedly at theĀ Ā 

play26:13

top of her forehead, giving herĀ  the aspect of an asylum inmate.ā€

play26:18

Susan Curtiss never really got caught upĀ  in the drama with Genie. The book she wroteĀ Ā 

play26:23

about Genieā€™s linguistic developments?Ā  She donated every dollar of the proceedsĀ Ā 

play26:29

to Genieā€™s estate. The woman who had spentĀ  so many hours, days, and years with Genie,Ā Ā 

play26:35

partly as a linguistic researcher, and mostly asĀ  a friend who took her on walks and shopping trips,Ā Ā 

play26:42

said, ā€œI missed her when she wasnā€™t in myĀ  lifeā€¦ I would pay a lot of money to see her,Ā Ā 

play26:50

I would do a lot. Iā€™d give up my job,Ā  Iā€™d change careers, to see her again.ā€

play26:58

But no one ever did.

play27:02

The last anyone knew, Genie was still alive,Ā Ā 

play27:06

somewhere in state care. She wouldĀ  have just turned 67 two months ago.

play27:13

And her name isnā€™t even Genie.

play27:18

She was called ā€œGenieā€ to protectĀ  her privacy as a minor -- thereā€™s noĀ Ā 

play27:23

information on who chose it or how itĀ  came to be. ā€œGenieā€ is the AnglicizedĀ Ā 

play27:27

version of the Arabic ā€˜jinn,ā€™ invisibleĀ  beings that can be either good or evil,Ā Ā 

play27:33

and who can be called upon for protectionĀ  or magical aid. Our modern tales haveĀ Ā 

play27:39

Genies appearing out of bottles and grantingĀ  a series of wishes to whomever releases them.

play27:46

Over the years, how many researchers andĀ  self-interested parties rubbed the bottle,Ā Ā 

play27:52

and then pressured Genie to grant their wishes?Ā  How many called upon a horrifically abused teenĀ Ā 

play27:59

to give them protection, and a kind of deusĀ  ex machina solution to their career ambitions?

play28:06

All while Genie herself needed toĀ  be protected and to have her wishesĀ Ā 

play28:12

granted -- her wish to speak, her wish toĀ  love and be loved, her wish to live a life.

play28:22

Wishes devastated by an abusive father,Ā  wishes exterminated in the name of science,Ā Ā 

play28:29

and a life dehumanized by a codename.

play28:34

Her name is Susan.

play28:38

As always, thanks for watching.

Rate This
ā˜…
ā˜…
ā˜…
ā˜…
ā˜…

5.0 / 5 (0 votes)

Related Tags
Feral ChildChild AbuseLinguisticsHuman TragedyScientific EthicsIsolation EffectsLanguage DevelopmentMental HealthHuman ExperimentCognitive Science