Does Input Have to Be "Comprehensible"?

Matt vs Japan
31 Aug 201814:35

Summary

TLDR本视频探讨了初学者学习日语时是否应该沉浸在自己能够理解的内容中。根据克拉申的输入假说,语言习得发生在理解之前无法理解的内容时。视频提出,对于初学者来说,理解90%的内容与理解30-40%的内容可能同样有效,甚至后者可能更有用。作者通过个人经验推测,即使只理解一小部分,也能通过不断接触新内容而快速进步。此外,强调了理解信息的重要性,而不仅仅是语法和词汇的每一个细节,并建议使用工具如维基百科和Netflix的剧集描述来提高理解度。

Takeaways

  • 📚 根据克拉申的输入假说,语言习得发生在你理解了之前不懂的内容时。
  • 🧩 理解是逐步的,可以部分理解句子而不理解全部,理解程度是连续的而非绝对的。
  • 🌱 作为日语初学者,不必只沉浸在你已经90%理解的内容中,30-40%的理解率可能更有用。
  • 🚀 即使只理解一小部分,大脑也会不断寻找并学习那些刚刚超出理解范围的新词汇和结构。
  • 🤔 没有科学研究确切表明理解率的最佳水平,但个人经验和理论表明,初学者从难度较高的内容中也能快速进步。
  • 🌐 理解信息比理解所有语法和词汇更为重要,克拉申强调理解信息是语言习得的关键。
  • 👶 婴儿学习母语时从0%理解开始,逐渐通过大量难以理解的成人语言输入学习,这表明大脑具备从不理解到理解的能力。
  • 🎯 学习者应该优先考虑材料的吸引力和参与度,而不是理解的百分比。
  • 🔍 使用工具如Wikipedia剧集摘要或Netflix的每集简介,可以帮助提高对正在观看内容的信息理解。
  • 🔁 理解信息的过程是无意识的,涉及复杂的大脑处理,而不必通过有意识的分析。

Q & A

  • 什么是克拉申的输入假说?

    -克拉申的输入假说认为,语言习得发生在你理解了之前不理解的内容时。通过上下文线索,比如理解句子中的其他部分,你可以推断出新词汇的含义,并因此习得这个词汇。

  • 为什么初学者在语言习得过程中可能会从理解30%的内容中受益?

    -初学者通过接触略高于他们水平的内容,可以不断地遇到新词汇和结构,这为他们提供了更多的机会去理解之前不理解的内容,从而促进语言习得。

  • 为什么理解90%的内容可能不如理解30%的内容有效?

    -理解90%的内容可能意味着只有10%的新信息,这限制了学习新内容的机会。相比之下,理解30%的内容可能意味着有70%的新信息,为学习提供了更多机会。

  • 为什么兴趣和参与度在语言习得中很重要?

    -兴趣和参与度可以增加学习者对材料的注意力和投入,这有助于提高语言习得的效率。如果学习者对材料不感兴趣,他们可能不会有足够的动力去理解和学习。

  • 什么是克拉申所说的'理解信息'?

    -克拉申所说的'理解信息'是指理解所传达的整体意义,而不仅仅是语法和词汇的每一个细节。即使某些词汇和语法结构超出了学习者的理解范围,他们仍然可以通过上下文线索理解信息。

  • 为什么说语言习得是一个非线性过程?

    -语言习得是一个非线性过程,因为大脑可以同时处理多个层面的信息,如语音、词汇和句法结构,而不是逐个逐步地学习。

  • 为什么说初学者在语言习得中不需要完全理解所有内容?

    -初学者不需要完全理解所有内容,因为他们的大脑有能力通过上下文和其他线索来推断和学习新词汇和结构,即使他们只理解部分内容。

  • 为什么说语言习得是一个潜意识过程?

    -克拉申认为语言习得是一个潜意识过程,意味着学习者不需要有意识地分析语言规则,而是通过不断的接触和使用语言,潜意识地吸收和习得语言知识。

  • 为什么说初学者应该使用他们感兴趣的材料来学习?

    -使用感兴趣的材料可以提高学习者的参与度和动机,这有助于他们更好地理解和习得语言。如果材料过于简单或无聊,学习者可能不会有足够的动力去学习。

  • 为什么说理解信息比理解每一个词汇和语法结构更重要?

    -理解信息可以帮助学习者抓住交流的主旨,这是语言交际的主要目的。而对每个词汇和语法结构的精确理解可以在理解信息的基础上逐渐发展。

Outlines

00:00

📚 初学者如何沉浸式学习日语

在这段视频中,讨论了初学者学习日语时是否应该沉浸在他们能够理解的内容中。根据史蒂芬·克拉申的输入假说,语言习得发生在你理解了之前不理解的内容时。视频通过举例说明,即使只理解部分内容,也能通过上下文线索来推断出整个句子的意思,从而习得语言。作者分享了自己学习日语的经验,认为对于初学者来说,理解30%到40%的内容可能比理解90%的内容更有用,因为这样可以提供更多的机会去理解之前不理解的内容。作者强调,理解是一个渐进的过程,不是非黑即白的,即使只理解一部分,也能从中学习到新的语言结构和词汇。

05:02

🧠 理解与语言习得的关系

这段内容深入探讨了理解与语言习得之间的关系。作者指出,理解并不是一个简单的是或否的问题,而是一个渐进的过程,可以是理解句子的整体意思,但不一定理解每个单词或语法结构。作者通过自己的学习经历,强调了即使在理解程度较低的情况下,也总是有机会接触到新的语言元素,并且这些新元素会不断重复出现,从而促进学习。作者认为,大脑有能力处理大量不理解的信息,并且能够从中学习,就像婴儿学习母语一样,从不理解逐渐过渡到理解。

10:02

🎯 初学者学习策略的探讨

在这部分中,作者讨论了初学者在学习日语时应采取的策略。作者认为,尽管没有科学研究明确指出哪种策略更有效,但根据个人经验和对语言习得的理解,作者推测初学者通过沉浸在难度较高的内容中可能会更快地进步。作者提出,即使只理解一小部分内容,也比完全理解的内容更有学习价值,因为这样可以不断接触到新的语言点。此外,作者还提到了保持学习材料的趣味性和参与度的重要性,因为动机和兴趣对于语言习得至关重要。最后,作者建议使用一些工具,如Wikipedia上的剧集简介,来增加对内容的理解,从而提高语言习得的效率。

Mindmap

Keywords

💡输入假说

输入假说(Input Hypothesis)是斯蒂芬·克拉申(Stephen Krashen)提出的一种语言习得理论,主张语言的习得是通过理解可理解输入(i+1)来实现的,即略高于学习者当前水平的语言输入。在视频中,输入假说被用来解释为何初学者在理解部分内容时仍能进行有效的语言学习,强调了即使只有部分理解,学习者也能通过上下文线索推断出新词汇或结构的含义。

💡i+1

i+1是输入假说中的一个核心概念,指的是学习者应该接收到略高于其当前语言水平的输入,以便能够理解和习得新的语言结构和词汇。视频中通过举例说明,即使是初学者,通过接触略高于他们理解水平的材料,也能够逐步提高语言能力。

💡可理解性

可理解性(Comprehensibility)在视频中指的是学习者能够理解的语言输入的程度。视频强调,即使是初学者,也应该寻找那些他们能够理解一部分的材料,因为这样的材料提供了学习新语言结构的机会。

💡沉浸式学习

沉浸式学习(Immersion)是指将学习者置于目标语言环境中,使他们不得不使用该语言进行交流和学习。视频中提到,即使是初学者,也应该尽可能地沉浸在目标语言中,即使他们一开始只能理解一小部分。

💡语言习得

语言习得(Language Acquisition)是指通过自然过程而非有意识学习获得语言能力。视频中讨论了语言习得与语言学习的区别,并强调了理解信息的重要性,以及如何通过理解来促进语言习得。

💡意识和无意识学习

意识和无意识学习(Conscious and Unconscious Learning)在视频中被提及,用来解释语言习得过程中意识和无意识的作用。视频指出,尽管学习者可能意识到他们不理解的部分,但无意识的大脑过程仍然在工作,帮助他们习得语言。

💡切割边缘

切割边缘(Cutting Edge)是克拉申理论中的一个概念,指的是学习者当前能够理解的语言水平的极限。视频中提到,学习者应该不断接触新的、略高于他们切割边缘的语言输入,以便推动语言能力的发展。

💡兴趣和参与度

兴趣和参与度(Interest and Engagement)在视频中被强调为语言学习的重要因素。视频指出,学习者应该选择他们感兴趣的材料进行学习,因为高度的参与度和兴趣可以提高语言习得的效率。

💡语言输入

语言输入(Language Input)是指学习者接收到的所有目标语言材料。视频中讨论了输入的质量对于语言习得的重要性,以及如何通过提供适当的输入来促进学习者的语言发展。

💡信息理解

信息理解(Message Comprehension)是指学习者能够理解语言输入的整体意义,而不必理解每一个词汇或语法结构。视频中提到,即使学习者只理解部分内容,也能够通过上下文线索理解整体信息,这对于语言习得是有益的。

Highlights

根据克拉申的输入假说,语言习得发生在你理解了之前不懂的内容时。

理解是习得语言的关键,而不仅仅是理解语法和词汇。

对于初学者来说,理解90%的内容与理解30-40%的内容在习得效率上可能没有显著差异。

即使只理解一小部分内容,大脑也能从中识别和学习新的语言结构。

理解的提高是一个渐进的过程,即使是从30%提高到31%也是进步。

大脑擅长从大量不理解的内容中识别出熟悉的模式和结构。

理解的深度不是简单的是或否,而是一个连续的过程。

即使只理解一小部分,也总是有机会学习新的内容。

大脑内置有从不理解到理解的策略,这是语言习得的关键。

对于初学者来说,理解程度较低的内容可能更有助于快速习得语言。

婴儿学习母语时也是从不理解开始,逐渐提高理解度。

理解信息比理解所有语法和词汇更重要。

使用工具如Wikipedia的剧集描述可以帮助提高对内容的理解。

参与度比理解程度更重要,应该优先考虑参与度。

克拉申强调理解信息的重要性,而不是语法和词汇的所有细节。

即使不能完全理解,通过上下文也能获取信息,这对语言习得是有帮助的。

总结来说,理解信息和参与度比理解的百分比更重要。

Transcripts

play00:00

so as a beginner learning Japanese

play00:02

should you be immersing yourself with

play00:03

content which is comprehensible to you

play00:06

well let's go into this a little bit so

play00:08

according to stephen krashen xinput

play00:10

hypothesis you acquire language when

play00:13

basically you comprehend something which

play00:16

you previously couldn't so if I hold up

play00:19

a ball a red ball and say the English

play00:22

sentence this is a red ball and let's

play00:25

say for example you didn't know the

play00:27

English word ball but you knew the rest

play00:28

of the sentence this is a red blank then

play00:31

you can probably make the inference oh

play00:32

the word ball probably means ball and

play00:36

now you just be due to the extra context

play00:39

clue of me holding up the red ball

play00:41

you're able to comprehend this sentence

play00:44

comprehend that the word ball means ball

play00:46

and then according to the input

play00:48

hypothesis you'll be able to acquire it

play00:49

so acquisition is all about

play00:51

understanding things which you

play00:53

previously couldn't understand so now

play00:57

when we're thinking about well should

play00:59

you be immersing in content which you

play01:01

understand like ninety to ninety percent

play01:03

of already or is it okay to immerse with

play01:07

content that you only understand 30 40

play01:10

50 percent of is one better than then

play01:12

the other are they about the same when

play01:14

we're thinking about this question we

play01:16

have to think about well which one will

play01:18

provide us with the most opportunities

play01:19

to comprehend things which we previously

play01:22

weren't able to comprehend well the

play01:25

reality is that I don't know and nobody

play01:27

really knows because there haven't been

play01:29

any sort of scientific studies conducted

play01:31

on this topic but I'm gonna talk to you

play01:33

about my speculations and my theories

play01:36

with this topic based off my own

play01:37

experience and my models of how language

play01:39

acquisition works so I actually don't

play01:44

think as a beginner of Japanese this is

play01:47

different once you become like late

play01:48

intermediate to advanced but as a

play01:50

beginner of Japanese I don't think that

play01:52

it's actually any better to be

play01:55

understanding 90% versus say 30 40 % and

play01:59

it might actually be even more useful to

play02:01

have 30 40 % when I was learning

play02:03

Japanese for the entire period where I

play02:05

was beginner I pretty much exclusively

play02:07

immersed with content which was way

play02:10

above my level you could say right I was

play02:12

only understanding

play02:13

probably 10 20 30 percent in the

play02:15

beginning later on like 50 60 70 percent

play02:18

for the first long while I almost never

play02:19

understood a hundred percent because I

play02:21

was watching anime and dramas and

play02:24

Japanese movies which I wanted to watch

play02:26

and I thought were cool I wasn't really

play02:27

thinking about was it gonna be too hard

play02:29

or not because everything was kind of

play02:31

too hard and I found that I made very

play02:34

rapid progress right it didn't seem to

play02:36

be a problem that I was only

play02:37

understanding 20 or 30 percent that

play02:39

didn't seem to be a barrier that kept me

play02:41

from progressing and the reason why this

play02:42

is is because like for example when I

play02:44

understood 30 percent of Japanese you

play02:46

know then there was certain sets of

play02:49

words and phrases which I had down pat

play02:52

every single time I heard it I knew what

play02:53

it meant like ah combing gonna eat you

play02:55

up

play02:55

so am I saying like stuff like that

play02:57

every time I heard it I instantly

play03:00

understood it I could not understand it

play03:01

right I had completely acquired it and

play03:03

my brain was very good at picking out

play03:04

those pieces of language from the whole

play03:07

and at any given point there was this

play03:09

new layer that was just beyond my

play03:11

cutting edge where I would start

play03:13

noticing these words or sentence

play03:16

structures right like one day I would

play03:18

notice some word like maybe like Kai

play03:22

said suit I would notice the word Kai

play03:24

Setsu and then once I noticed it once

play03:26

I'd be like oh yeah but kind of sounds

play03:28

familiar I feel like I've probably heard

play03:30

that before I started noticing it all

play03:31

the time like multiple times per day

play03:33

because I mean that's just how it works

play03:34

with the baader-meinhof phenomenon right

play03:37

once you notice things once then you

play03:39

just start noticing it all over the

play03:40

place you know you're thinking about

play03:41

getting a certain type of car suddenly

play03:43

you start seeing that car everywhere you

play03:45

go even though you never noticed it

play03:47

before you thought about maybe buying

play03:48

that car that's just how kind of the

play03:50

brain works and so once your brain

play03:52

unlocks a word and this would happen all

play03:55

the time right because your brain just

play03:57

starts noticing things your brain is

play03:58

always starting to notice new language

play04:01

structures and patterns which are just

play04:03

beyond your cutting edge and so I would

play04:05

notice words like this

play04:06

I use the example concepts I would just

play04:08

notice where it's left or right all the

play04:09

time every day there'd be three four or

play04:11

five new words which I noticed for the

play04:13

first time that day and once I I notice

play04:16

notice them I noticed start noticing

play04:17

them all the time multiple times per day

play04:19

usually and so it was inevitable that I

play04:22

learned the meaning of those words right

play04:23

because your custom that often

play04:25

eventually you just figure out what they

play04:26

mean

play04:27

or even if you don't wait until you

play04:28

actually figured out the context you can

play04:30

just look it up once get the meaning and

play04:32

then you can't forget it because it just

play04:34

gets reinforced every time it comes up

play04:35

it's like a natural SRS and so I had no

play04:38

trouble going from 30 percent to 31

play04:41

percent and from 31 to 32 and it didn't

play04:44

matter that I was only at 30% and there

play04:46

was actually a majority that I wasn't

play04:48

understanding because I was constantly

play04:51

improving what I was understanding all

play04:53

the time and here's the thing with

play04:55

comprehensibility is that it's not a

play04:57

yes-or-no thing right it's not like you

play04:59

hundred percent understood an entire

play05:01

sentence it's it's overall meaning how

play05:04

the grammar works what the individual

play05:06

vocab means it and you either understood

play05:07

it yes or it was all gibberish to you

play05:09

right that's not how it works

play05:11

you can understand the general gist of a

play05:14

sentence but not really know what some

play05:17

of the individual words mean or how the

play05:19

grammar is interacting you can

play05:21

understand half of a sentence and note

play05:23

okay this word this partisan this means

play05:25

this that this is happening

play05:26

grammatically but not know the other

play05:28

half of the sentence right so

play05:29

understanding is a very messy thing it's

play05:31

not a yes/no type of type of deal and so

play05:35

even if you're only understanding 30% of

play05:37

like the overall language there's still

play05:39

gonna be language that's on your cutting

play05:41

edge all around you all the time like a

play05:43

few months ago when I was learning

play05:44

Chinese I do is just immersing for like

play05:46

a few months and I was still at a very

play05:48

low level like only understanding 20-30

play05:50

percent but there was like almost

play05:52

something from for my brain to do on

play05:54

almost every sentence because there was

play05:56

either one word in each sentence that

play05:58

sounded familiar and I was thinking wait

play06:00

what does that mean I feel like I've

play06:01

heard that before do I know that word or

play06:03

like some grammatical structure where I

play06:04

was like oh wait was that the structure

play06:06

I heard before like how is this going

play06:07

here right so even though that I was

play06:09

only understanding 30% of the total hole

play06:10

I was constantly bombarded with

play06:13

opportunities to learn new things and

play06:15

that was what what's important right

play06:17

what I mentioned at the beginning is how

play06:20

much opportunities are you being given

play06:22

to comprehend things which you weren't

play06:25

able to comprehend before so it's this

play06:27

idea that going from 30 to 31 is it's

play06:30

the same amount of progress as going

play06:32

from 95 to 96 and it's not necessarily

play06:34

harder to do it's actually in a sense

play06:36

easier to do because you're being

play06:38

bombarded with so much that you don't

play06:40

understand right

play06:41

if you understand 98% of something and

play06:43

you don't only understand 2% then you

play06:45

only are presented with the opportunity

play06:47

to learn that 2% and yes it might be

play06:50

easier to learn that 2% because it's a

play06:52

clear or what it means because you

play06:54

everything surrounding the 2% is already

play06:56

clear but if you only understand 50%

play06:58

then half of all you're listening to is

play07:01

stuff that's new to you and you have the

play07:02

potential to pick up so in a way you're

play07:05

you have more opportunities to learn and

play07:07

like I said it's not very hard to pick

play07:10

up things that are right beyond your

play07:11

cutting edge and so I think having the

play07:13

extra exposure to constant you don't

play07:15

understand at the beginning stages might

play07:17

actually lead to you picking up more

play07:20

more quickly because when you think

play07:22

about it babies who are learning their

play07:24

l1 they start out with a 0%

play07:26

comprehension right they're not

play07:27

understanding 98% filling of the 2% they

play07:30

understand nothing at the beginning and

play07:31

they slowly work their way up given

play07:33

completely incomprehensible mostly adult

play07:35

input right like yeah adults speak to

play07:37

babies and baby-talk but that's like 2%

play07:39

or less of all their input most of their

play07:42

learning happens through hearing adult

play07:44

conversation and so the brain is kind of

play07:47

built it has strategies built in to

play07:50

learn how to work its way up the

play07:52

comprehension hierarchy right from 0 to

play07:55

1 2 3 4 7 5% physics percent your brain

play07:57

has the tools it needs to do that

play07:59

built-in and because the unconscious

play08:01

mind because you know language

play08:02

acquisition happens unconsciously that's

play08:05

what stephen krashen tells us and what

play08:06

my experience tells me your unconscious

play08:10

mind doesn't work in a strict linear

play08:12

fashion like your conscious mind does it

play08:14

can be working on multiple issues at

play08:16

once it can be working on multiple

play08:17

layers of abstraction at once and so at

play08:20

the very beginning part of your brain

play08:22

could be working on phonetics and

play08:24

parsing the differences between little

play08:26

sounds part of your brain could be

play08:27

dealing with what individual words mean

play08:29

part of your brain could be dealing with

play08:31

the sentence structure and this could be

play08:32

happening simultaneously right in a kind

play08:34

of parallel processing fashion because

play08:36

we know that the brain does process

play08:38

things like many multiple things at the

play08:40

same time like psychology tells us this

play08:42

and so like at the very beginning right

play08:44

if you're only exposing yourself to what

play08:46

you already understood that what would

play08:48

that be like hello this is a pen and you

play08:51

have such a limited range of input

play08:53

that's all the other

play08:54

the structures in your brain which are

play08:56

equipped to decode full-on real language

play09:00

and and extract the underlying

play09:02

principles from that input it's not

play09:04

going to get a chance to really activate

play09:06

if your input is so thin and easy right

play09:09

so I think that you can work with your

play09:12

brain by replicating the process of

play09:14

first language acquisition of basically

play09:16

doing what your brain was built to do

play09:18

which is figure out how to swim while

play09:21

it's drowning you could say but then

play09:23

again at the end of the day this is just

play09:25

my hypothesis based off my own

play09:26

experience I mean what I can tell you is

play09:28

that if you immerse with things that

play09:31

only you only understand 30 40 % of you

play09:33

will get good and pretty quickly because

play09:36

me and my other other people I know who

play09:38

did a jet got good very quickly never

play09:41

using basically watered down baby

play09:43

materials but I can't say definitively

play09:45

that's the better strategy maybe who

play09:48

knows hypothetically if you always

play09:50

understood 90% of your input or 95% of

play09:53

effort then you would get better even

play09:54

faster because the it'd be easier for

play09:57

your brain to learn what was missing I

play09:58

can't say that for sure but here are

play10:00

some potential problems that strike me

play10:02

when I think about trying to think that

play10:03

other strategy I mean the biggest issue

play10:06

is that I mean when you're true beginner

play10:08

no content is gonna be like 90%

play10:10

comprehensible right except if it was

play10:12

like literally baby talk of just

play10:13

individual words but I mean once you get

play10:16

past that space right it's like what

play10:17

Dora the Explorer Blue's Clues like

play10:20

those are the shows that we're talking

play10:21

about and for most adults that's

play10:23

extremely boring and tedious to watch

play10:25

and engagement is extremely important

play10:27

stephen krashen

play10:28

and Steve Kaufman talked to talk about

play10:30

this all the time your attitude and your

play10:32

engagement and your motivation to

play10:34

comprehend what you're listening to and

play10:36

your actual interest in it greatly

play10:38

depend how much you're going to acquire

play10:39

and so even if hypothetically going from

play10:43

95 to 96 was more efficient than going

play10:46

from 30 to 31 which I said there's no

play10:49

real reason to assume that that's the

play10:51

case but even if it were then it still

play10:53

might be the better strategy for to go

play10:55

from 30 to 31 if that allows you to be

play10:57

immersing with content that you're

play10:58

actually engaged with right because it's

play11:01

pretty much extremely unlikely that

play11:02

you're gonna get anywhere if you're

play11:03

forcing yourself to immerse with

play11:05

materials that you don't like unless

play11:08

you're

play11:08

meditation master which pretty much none

play11:10

of us are and then besides interest

play11:12

right like how are you going to find

play11:14

these materials like that would mean

play11:15

that you're constantly looking for the

play11:17

material that's perfectly adjusted to

play11:19

your level even though you're getting

play11:20

better all the time and I think that

play11:22

it's not a very realistic strategy in a

play11:24

real life right because that means that

play11:26

you're gonna be have to spend so much

play11:29

time chasing down the perfect material

play11:30

that you're gonna be spending more time

play11:32

looking for materials than actually just

play11:33

immersing with them so that's kind of my

play11:36

main thoughts on this topic what I do

play11:38

think is that you know stephen krashen

play11:40

when he was talking about

play11:41

comprehensibility he was talking about

play11:43

understanding messages he emphasized

play11:45

that a lot he wasn't talking about you

play11:47

understand all the nuances of grammar

play11:48

and vocab like if you look at some of

play11:51

the lectures he's given on YouTube he

play11:54

will give the example of two different

play11:56

lessons that he gives in German or

play11:58

something and one of them he just speaks

play11:59

German and the other one he's like

play12:00

showing you like with visual clues what

play12:02

he means but he's still speaking in full

play12:04

sentences right he's speaking on full

play12:05

blown German in that example just

play12:08

because of the hand motions and the and

play12:10

emphasizing certain parts of the

play12:12

sentence you can tell what the overall

play12:14

meaning is and so like I said before

play12:16

that is it a difference between

play12:17

understanding the message of what's

play12:19

being said and understanding actually

play12:20

the grammar in the vocab right like you

play12:23

from just context alone even if you have

play12:25

the sound off you can get the message of

play12:26

what's being said in certain situations

play12:28

even if the vocab and grammar is beyond

play12:30

you and I think that's what chris

play12:32

stephen krashen emphasized as the most

play12:34

important component for acquisition

play12:35

because again like figuring out the

play12:37

grammar and the vocab that happens

play12:39

unconsciously through like extremely

play12:41

complicated inductive computational

play12:43

processes that are way beyond what we

play12:45

can do with our conscious brain right so

play12:47

basically I think what stephen krashen

play12:49

was saying is to engage those processes

play12:51

you have to understand the message of

play12:53

what's being said and then the

play12:54

unconscious mind will do all the rest

play12:56

and I do think there is something to

play12:58

that and so that's why I think for

play13:00

example watching a TV show that you have

play13:02

watched in your native language before

play13:04

like watching the Japanese dub of your

play13:06

favorite American movie watching an

play13:07

anime that you had watched before with

play13:09

English subtitles

play13:10

washing the raw without the subtitles or

play13:12

even going on Wikipedia and reading the

play13:15

episode descriptions of each episode

play13:16

before you actually watch the episode

play13:18

that can help a lot too or even on

play13:21

Netflix you know they give you

play13:22

each episode has a little paragraph

play13:24

description of what happened in the

play13:25

episode you can read that beforehand and

play13:27

then get it just of what's happening and

play13:29

so these are tools you can use to make

play13:31

the message of what you're watching more

play13:34

comprehensible and I do think there's a

play13:35

lot of value in that I think that is

play13:37

more important than actual

play13:38

percentage-wise of how many words are

play13:40

you understanding and things like that

play13:41

and so to summarize in conclusion I

play13:44

think that although we can't know for

play13:46

sure which is the more optimal strategy

play13:49

having almost full comprehensibility and

play13:52

filling in the last spot or just having

play13:54

less comprehensibility and working on

play13:55

your cutting edge but we know that that

play13:58

method at least has to work because it's

play14:00

worked for me and on many other people

play14:02

that's like the basis of the original

play14:04

agent philosophy so it's proven to work

play14:06

then we know it works pretty quickly at

play14:08

them at the very least and in a way your

play14:11

engagement with the material is much

play14:12

more important than the

play14:13

comprehensibility so I think I can say

play14:15

pretty definitively that no matter what

play14:18

you end up watching you should

play14:19

prioritize your engagement over how much

play14:22

you're understanding but when you can

play14:25

using the tools I talked about before

play14:27

like episode summaries on Wikipedia can

play14:29

help make things more comprehensible in

play14:31

a message kind of way and I do think

play14:32

that is value so yeah

Rate This

5.0 / 5 (0 votes)

Related Tags
日语学习沉浸式理解语言习得Krashen理论输入假说学习策略初学者进阶学习语言理解
Do you need a summary in English?