Forms of Reliability in Research and Statistics

Daniel Storage
24 Jun 201911:46

Summary

TLDRThis video delves into the concept of reliability in statistics and research, emphasizing its importance for making accurate inferences about populations. It discusses four types of reliability: test-retest, parallel forms, inter-rater, and internal consistency. Test-retest reliability assesses consistency over time, parallel forms reliability compares two versions of a test, inter-rater reliability measures agreement between observers, and internal consistency evaluates the coherence of items within a scale. The video illustrates these concepts with examples, highlighting the need for high reliability scores to ensure accurate predictions and minimize error.

Takeaways

  • 📏 Reliability in statistics refers to the consistency of measurement, which is crucial for making accurate inferences and conclusions in research.
  • 🔄 Test-retest reliability measures the stability of a test or measurement tool over time by administering it twice and comparing the results.
  • 📋 Parallel forms reliability assesses whether two different versions of a test (forms A and B) are equivalent in their measurement of the same construct.
  • 👥 Inter-rater reliability determines the level of agreement between two or more raters evaluating the same phenomenon, which is vital in observational research.
  • 🔗 Internal consistency checks if the items within a scale or test measure a single construct consistently, ensuring the scale's reliability.
  • 📉 A strong positive correlation close to 1 indicates good reliability, while values close to 0 or negative values suggest poor reliability.
  • 🤔 The example of an IQ test illustrates how test-retest reliability works, with scores expected to be similar if the test is reliable.
  • 📉 In the case of parallel forms reliability, the correlation between form A and form B should be high, indicating they measure the same construct equally well.
  • 👍 High inter-rater reliability, often expressed as a percentage agreement, shows that raters are consistent in their observations.
  • ⚖️ Internal consistency is calculated using a specific formula and is important for validating newly developed scales or tests in psychological research.
  • 📈 Improving reliability reduces measurement error and helps align research findings more closely with the true state of the population or phenomenon being studied.

Q & A

  • What is the basic definition of reliability in statistics and research?

    -Reliability refers to the consistency of measurement. It ensures that the measurements taken are stable and consistent over time, which is crucial for making accurate inferences in research.

  • Why is reliability important when progressing to inferential statistics?

    -Reliability is important because inconsistent measurements can lead to inaccurate conclusions about populations or the world. Consistency in data allows for more reliable inferences in research.

  • What are the four types of reliability discussed in the video?

    -The four types of reliability discussed are test-retest reliability, parallel forms reliability, inter-rater reliability, and internal consistency.

  • How is test-retest reliability assessed?

    -Test-retest reliability is assessed by giving the same test to the same participants at two different times and measuring the correlation between the scores. A strong positive correlation indicates good test-retest reliability.

  • Can you give an example of good and poor test-retest reliability?

    -Good test-retest reliability is when scores from time 1 and time 2 are similar, such as a participant scoring 100 on an IQ test at time 1 and 101 at time 2. Poor reliability is when scores differ significantly, like scoring 98 at time 1 and 115 at time 2.

  • What is parallel forms reliability, and how does it differ from test-retest reliability?

    -Parallel forms reliability examines the consistency between two different forms of the same test. Unlike test-retest reliability, which uses the same test twice, parallel forms involve two different versions to assess if they are equally reliable.

  • How do you measure inter-rater reliability?

    -Inter-rater reliability is measured by calculating the percentage of agreement between two or more observers or experimenters. It reflects how consistent different observers are in their judgments.

  • What is an example of inter-rater reliability, and how is it calculated?

    -An example of inter-rater reliability is two experimenters counting smiles in a study. If they agree on 8 out of 10 trials, the inter-rater reliability is 80%. It’s calculated as the number of agreements divided by the total number of trials.

  • What does internal consistency measure?

    -Internal consistency measures whether the items on a scale or test are consistent with each other, ensuring they are all measuring the same concept or construct.

  • Can you provide an example of poor internal consistency in a scale?

    -An example of poor internal consistency is a scale with some items measuring anxiety (e.g., 'I often feel nervous') and other items measuring depression (e.g., 'I no longer take pleasure in things I used to enjoy'). The mixed focus leads to low internal consistency.

Outlines

00:00

🔄 Understanding Reliability in Research and Statistics

This paragraph introduces the concept of reliability in statistics and research, emphasizing the importance of consistent and accurate measurements. Inconsistent measurements can lead to faulty conclusions about populations and the world. The paragraph outlines four types of reliability: test-retest reliability, parallel forms reliability, inter-rater reliability, and internal consistency, which all revolve around the idea of consistency.

05:01

📊 Test-Retest Reliability: Consistency Over Time

Test-retest reliability measures the consistency of a test over time. This paragraph explains how it works: individuals take the same test twice, and a correlation is calculated between their scores at both times. The higher the correlation, the more reliable the test. A good example is shown with IQ scores, where consistent results across time demonstrate high test-retest reliability, while a significant variation shows poor reliability.

10:03

📋 Parallel Forms Reliability: Comparing Different Test Versions

Parallel forms reliability checks the consistency between two different versions of the same test. This method is useful for teachers or researchers who use different forms of an exam or assessment. The paragraph explains how it's measured: by calculating a correlation between scores on both forms of the test. High correlation means the forms are equally reliable in assessing the same concepts.

👥 Inter-Rater Reliability: Agreement Among Observers

Inter-rater reliability assesses how much two or more observers or raters agree on their observations. It's particularly relevant in observational research, where consistency between multiple observers ensures accurate representation of outcomes. This is calculated by the percentage of agreements divided by the total possible agreements. An example using children's smiles illustrates how inter-rater reliability is determined.

📐 Internal Consistency: Measuring a Single Construct

Internal consistency determines whether items on a test or scale are measuring the same concept. The paragraph highlights how this form of reliability is crucial for scales, particularly in psychological research. The example of an anxiety scale shows how items measuring different concepts, like depression, can lead to poor internal consistency. Internal consistency is typically measured using a correlation, where higher values indicate more reliable measures.

🔢 Why Reliability Matters: Reducing Error in Research

The final paragraph ties together the importance of reliability in research, noting that consistent measurements are crucial for making accurate predictions and conclusions about populations. Higher reliability reduces errors, bringing estimates closer to the truth. The paragraph also teases the importance of validity, to be discussed in future content, in ensuring that measurements align with what researchers intend to measure.

Mindmap

Keywords

💡Reliability

Reliability in the context of the video refers to the consistency of measurement in statistics and research. It is crucial for ensuring that the conclusions drawn from data are accurate and consistent over time. The video emphasizes the importance of reliability because without it, inferences and predictions about populations or phenomena could be flawed. For instance, the video discusses test-retest reliability, which is measured by administering the same test twice and checking for consistency in the results.

💡Test-retest reliability

Test-retest reliability is a specific type of reliability that assesses whether a test or measurement tool yields consistent results over time. The video uses the example of an IQ test to illustrate this concept, explaining that if the test is reliable, a person's score should be similar when they take the test on two different occasions. The video provides a hypothetical dataset to demonstrate how scores might align closely, indicating strong test-retest reliability.

💡Parallel forms reliability

Parallel forms reliability is a concept introduced in the video to determine if two different versions of the same test, or 'forms', are equivalent in their measurement. This is important in educational assessments where multiple versions of a test might be used. The video explains that this type of reliability is measured by administering two different forms of a test to the same group of individuals and then correlating the scores from both forms to see if they are similar.

💡Inter-rater reliability

Inter-rater reliability is discussed in the video as a measure of the consistency between different raters or observers when assessing the same phenomenon. It is particularly relevant in observational research where multiple observers might be used to ensure a comprehensive view of the subject. The video provides an example of measuring happiness in children by counting smiles, and how the agreement between two experimenters on the number of smiles observed would indicate the level of inter-rater reliability.

💡Internal consistency

Internal consistency is a method of assessing the reliability of a scale or test by examining whether its items are consistent with each other. The video explains that this type of reliability is crucial when developing new scales for psychological research. It gives an example of an anxiety scale where some items might actually measure depression, leading to poor internal consistency because they do not all measure the same construct.

💡Correlation

Correlation is a statistical measure used in the video to quantify the degree to which two variables are linearly related. It is used to calculate test-retest reliability and parallel forms reliability by finding the correlation between scores from different testing occasions or different forms of a test. The video mentions that a strong positive correlation indicates high reliability.

💡Measurement

Measurement in the video refers to the process of assigning numbers or symbols to attributes of objects or events according to certain rules. It is a fundamental concept in research, especially in the context of reliability. The video discusses how consistent and accurate measurements are necessary for making reliable inferences about populations.

💡Inferential statistics

Inferential statistics are mentioned in the video as a branch of statistics that deals with making predictions or inferences about populations based on sample data. The video highlights that reliability is essential for inferential statistics because without consistent and accurate measurements, the conclusions drawn from these inferences could be incorrect.

💡Validity

Validity, while not the main focus of the video, is mentioned in passing as another important aspect of research alongside reliability. Validity refers to whether a test or measurement tool is actually measuring what it is supposed to measure. The video suggests that while reliability is about consistency, validity is about the accuracy of the measurement in relation to the construct it aims to assess.

💡Error

Error in the video is discussed in the context of measurement inaccuracies that can lead to unreliable results. The video explains that increasing reliability decreases error, which in turn brings estimates and predictions closer to the truth. Error is an undesired component of measurement that can arise from various sources, such as instrument imprecision or human error.

💡Scale

A scale in the video refers to a set of items or questions used to measure a particular psychological construct, such as anxiety or depression. The video discusses how developing a reliable scale is important for psychological research and that internal consistency is a method used to assess the reliability of these scales.

Highlights

Reliability in statistics is crucial for consistent and accurate measurements.

Inaccurate measurements can lead to false conclusions in inferential statistics.

Four types of reliability are discussed: test-retest, parallel forms, inter-rater, and internal consistency.

Test-retest reliability measures consistency over time using the same test.

Parallel forms reliability examines the equivalence of two different forms of the same test.

Inter-rater reliability assesses the agreement between different raters or observers.

Internal consistency measures whether items on a scale are consistent with each other.

A strong positive correlation indicates good test-retest reliability.

Parallel forms reliability is measured by correlating scores on two different forms of a test.

Inter-rater reliability is calculated as the percentage of agreement between raters.

Internal consistency is more complex to calculate and requires a specific formula.

An example of poor test-retest reliability is shown with significant score variation over time.

An example of good test-retest reliability is demonstrated with scores clustering closely together.

A practical example of inter-rater reliability is given using observations of children's smiles.

An example of a poorly constructed anxiety scale is critiqued for mixing anxiety and depression symptoms.

High reliability scores close to 1 indicate excellent consistency in measurements.

The importance of reliability for making accurate predictions and estimations is emphasized.

Transcripts

play00:00

in this video we're gonna talk about

play00:02

reliability in statistics and research

play00:04

reliability is a simple concept it's

play00:06

essentially your consistency of

play00:08

measurement and this is important

play00:10

because we need to be taking consistent

play00:13

measurements and accurate measurements

play00:15

of the world or else as we progress on

play00:17

to inferential statistics we're going to

play00:19

end up making inaccurate conclusions

play00:21

about populations inaccurate conclusions

play00:24

about the world so today I'm gonna talk

play00:26

to you about four different types of

play00:28

reliability and you're gonna see this

play00:29

idea of consistency of measurement sort

play00:32

of underlying all of them although they

play00:34

will take slightly different forms so

play00:37

first we're going to talk about test

play00:38

retest reliability

play00:39

next we'll talk about parallel forms

play00:42

reliability then inter-rater reliability

play00:44

and finally internal consistency a

play00:48

little bit of a different one so let's

play00:50

start with test retest reliability

play00:53

test retest reliability as the name

play00:56

suggests is used when you want to

play00:58

determine whether a test or a scale or

play01:00

some psychological measurement tool or

play01:02

whatever is reliable over time and the

play01:06

idea is that you're gonna test people

play01:07

and then you're going to retest them and

play01:09

you're gonna see if scores align are

play01:11

your scores consistent over time and

play01:14

this is typically measured as a simple

play01:16

correlation which you already know how

play01:18

to calculate from previous videos so

play01:20

it's a correlation between how people

play01:22

score at time one when they first take

play01:25

the test and those same participants how

play01:27

they score time two when they take the

play01:29

test again so let me illustrate with an

play01:32

example let's say I want to develop a

play01:33

new IQ test well if my IQ test is

play01:36

actually doing a good job of measuring

play01:38

people's IQ we would expect people to

play01:40

score similarly the first time they take

play01:42

the test and the second time they take

play01:44

the test so let's look at some sample

play01:46

data here so let's say these are scores

play01:48

at time one and these are scores at time

play01:50

two of the same participants so if you

play01:52

took a quick scan here you'll see we're

play01:54

doing pretty well let's say it's like a

play01:55

month later when they take the test

play01:57

again so here participant 1 starts out

play01:59

with an IQ of 100 right at average and

play02:02

they end up with an IQ of 101 very

play02:04

similar and across all these

play02:06

participants you're gonna see we're

play02:08

typically only one or two points off you

play02:10

know here's a little bit of a bigger

play02:12

difference maybe just an Coffee this

play02:14

morning or whatever right but overall we

play02:17

would say that this has good test retest

play02:20

reliability the scores tend to cluster

play02:22

together very closely and if you

play02:25

actually did the correlation between

play02:26

these two variables you would get an

play02:28

extremely strong correlation 0.99 almost

play02:32

a perfect relationship between how

play02:34

people do in the beginning to how people

play02:36

do at the end now here's an example of

play02:39

not so great test retest reliability

play02:41

you're gonna see for example look a

play02:43

participant number three the first time

play02:45

they took the IQ test they scored 98

play02:47

slightly below average the second time

play02:50

though a month later they scored a 115

play02:53

above average this is actually one

play02:55

standard deviation above the mean and

play02:58

this is an example of scores varying

play03:01

wildly from time one to time two and we

play03:04

wouldn't expect this to happen right

play03:06

there's no reason to believe that within

play03:07

a month someone would increase their

play03:09

intelligence by this much it's simply

play03:12

not feasible a better alternative

play03:13

explanation is that my IQ test is simply

play03:16

not a good IQ test and by the way if you

play03:19

did the correlation between these two

play03:20

variables it would look pretty pathetic

play03:22

negative 0.08 very poor test retest

play03:25

reliability

play03:27

so next let's talk about parallel forms

play03:29

reliability parallel forms reliability

play03:32

is very similar but it's sort of a more

play03:34

specific more unique case of test retest

play03:37

reliability

play03:38

parallel forms reliability is used when

play03:41

you want to examine the equivalence or

play03:43

similarity between two forms of the same

play03:45

test so for example if you're a teacher

play03:48

and you have a form a and a form beyond

play03:50

the exam you might want to know if those

play03:52

forms are equally difficult if they're

play03:54

doing a good job of you know assessing

play03:56

the same concepts things like that are

play03:59

they similar to one another and you're

play04:02

gonna measure parallel forms reliability

play04:04

in a similar sort of way as we did with

play04:06

test retest reliability

play04:07

you're gonna give people form a in the

play04:09

beginning maybe a week later or a month

play04:11

later at time two you'll give them form

play04:14

B so the only difference here we're

play04:16

still measuring test and retest but the

play04:18

difference is we have two different

play04:20

forms it's not a copy and paste of the

play04:22

same test twice which is what we have

play04:25

with test retest reliability

play04:27

so four parallel forms reliability

play04:29

you're also gonna measure it the same

play04:30

way as with test retest reliability it's

play04:34

just gonna be a simple correlation

play04:35

between scores for the same individuals

play04:37

on form a and form B at these two

play04:40

different time points and again we're

play04:42

gonna hope for a strong positive

play04:44

correlation we're gonna hope that scores

play04:46

tend to be similar on form a as on Form

play04:49

B

play04:51

next we have inter-rater reliability

play04:53

this one is used for a slightly

play04:55

different situation but it's still an

play04:57

idea of consistency underlying it

play05:00

interrater reliability is used when you

play05:03

want to know how much two different

play05:05

raters or experimenters or observers

play05:07

agree on their judgments of an outcome

play05:10

of interest so there are many cases in

play05:12

which you might be interested in

play05:13

integrator to reliability but it's

play05:15

definitely something that's most

play05:16

prevalent in observational research so

play05:19

typically if you're observing say a

play05:21

child or developmental psychologist

play05:23

maybe you're not just gonna observe that

play05:25

child alone right you're gonna use

play05:26

multiple observers multiple expenditures

play05:29

because people can miss things you may

play05:31

not notice something right so it's

play05:33

better to have multiple observers to

play05:34

really make sure you're getting true and

play05:36

accurate representation of what happened

play05:39

and this is what inter-rater reliability

play05:40

is all about it's about are those

play05:43

different experimenters consistent with

play05:45

one another in terms of what they're

play05:47

seeing do they tend to agree with one

play05:48

another so there is sort of a formula

play05:51

for inter-rater reliability and here it

play05:53

is it's almost not necessary though

play05:55

because it's just a simple percentage

play05:57

agreement that's it it's a proportion or

play05:59

a percentage of the number of times that

play06:01

the two experimenters or more are

play06:04

agreeing with one another so it's

play06:06

interrater reliability equaling the

play06:08

number of times the experimenters agreed

play06:10

with one another divided by the number

play06:13

of times they could have possibly agreed

play06:15

if they were perfect so this is sort of

play06:17

the number of trials and again this is

play06:19

kind of a percentage so let's take an

play06:21

example

play06:22

let's say I'm interested in happiness

play06:25

right measuring happiness among children

play06:27

maybe I'm interested in gender

play06:29

differences and how happy boys and girls

play06:31

are and this is sort of my starting

play06:32

point so I'm gonna have two

play06:34

experimenters observe how often a child

play06:36

smiles this is how I'm gonna

play06:37

operationalize happiness how I'm gonna

play06:40

define it and make observable so how

play06:42

often does this child smile across ten

play06:45

one-minute time intervals so let's say I

play06:48

do this study I collect this data here's

play06:51

my data for experimenter one and here it

play06:53

is for experiment or to all ten trials

play06:56

and the number of smiles each

play06:57

experimenter saw so you'll notice that

play07:00

in general they tend to agree pretty

play07:02

well on trial one experimenter one saw

play07:05

two smiles as did experimenter two and

play07:07

so on but you'll notice that two of

play07:10

these are disagreements on trial for for

play07:13

example experimenter one saw four smells

play07:15

whereas experimenter two only saw three

play07:18

and we see disagreement on trial seven

play07:20

as well so in this case we have eight

play07:23

agreements and two disagreements so our

play07:27

inter-rater reliability is simply 8 over

play07:31

10 because that's the number of trials

play07:34

that's the number of possible times they

play07:36

could have agreed if they were perfect

play07:37

so in this case we're gonna have 8 over

play07:40

10 or 80% 0.8 if you'd like to think of

play07:43

this as a proportion and this is our

play07:45

interrater reliability

play07:48

so finally we have an internal

play07:51

consistency internal consistency is a

play07:53

pretty simple idea it is kind of a pain

play07:55

to calculate unlike some of these others

play07:57

which are just simple correlations or

play07:59

you know just a percentage basically

play08:02

internal consistency has its own formula

play08:05

which we'll talk about in the next video

play08:06

it has its own sort of process and it is

play08:09

quite laborious to actually compute but

play08:11

totally manageable if you follow some

play08:13

steps that I'm gonna go over again in

play08:15

the next video but for now let's just

play08:17

think conceptually about what internal

play08:19

consistency really is okay so many times

play08:22

in psychological research you're gonna

play08:24

need to measure something that hasn't

play08:25

been measured very often if at all in

play08:27

the past and often times the best way to

play08:30

kind of go about this problems to

play08:32

develop a scale you'll give participants

play08:34

a series of items and you'll ask them to

play08:36

rate their agreement to those items

play08:38

those statements for example on a one to

play08:40

nine scale one perhaps being strongly

play08:43

disagree and nine being strongly agree a

play08:46

pretty standard sort of scale now if

play08:48

you're gonna develop your own scale and

play08:50

you want to publish those results for

play08:51

example you're going to need to prove to

play08:53

experts in the field other professors

play08:55

and graduate students and so on that

play08:58

your scale is reliable and also a talked

play09:02

about in a future video that your scale

play09:04

is valid so internal consistency is a

play09:06

way of measuring the reliability of a

play09:09

scale it's used when you want to know

play09:11

whether items on a scale or a test or

play09:13

whatever are consistent with each other

play09:15

showing that they measure one and only

play09:18

one thing so here's an example of an

play09:20

anxiety scale that I developed for the

play09:22

purposes of this video let's take a look

play09:25

at each of these items and kind of make

play09:26

a guess about what the internal

play09:28

consistency is gonna look like

play09:30

so item one is I often have worrying

play09:33

thoughts item two I have trouble getting

play09:36

out of bed in the morning I'm three I

play09:38

often feel nervous item four I no longer

play09:42

take pleasure in things I used to enjoy

play09:44

item five my heart often beats fast as

play09:48

fear enters in and item six I often feel

play09:51

sluggish and tired so do you notice any

play09:54

potential problems with this anxiety

play09:56

scale well you might have noticed that

play09:59

items 1 3 & 5 measure anxiety whereas

play10:02

items 2 4 & 6 are actually doing a

play10:05

better job of getting at depression I

play10:07

have trouble getting out of bed in the

play10:09

morning I no longer take pleasure and

play10:11

things I used to enjoy this is what we

play10:12

call Antonia and I often feel sluggish

play10:16

and tired these are all symptoms of

play10:18

depression so in this case you can

play10:19

imagine if a person with anxiety takes

play10:22

this test takes this scale they're gonna

play10:24

respond one way to items 1 3 & 5 but if

play10:28

they don't have depression they're gonna

play10:29

respond very differently into items 2 4

play10:31

& 6 so all six items will not really do

play10:34

a great job of working together and the

play10:36

result here is going to be poor internal

play10:39

consistency

play10:40

so just in general we want reliability

play10:43

scores to be positive we don't want

play10:45

negative scores remember a lot of these

play10:47

are correlations for example we want

play10:50

strong positive correlations and we want

play10:52

those values to be as large as possible

play10:54

typically between 0 & 1 although you can

play10:57

have values outside that range but we

play10:59

want values close to 1 you're gonna see

play11:02

this when we calculate the internal

play11:03

consistency as well just as one example

play11:05

an internal consistency of 0.95 is

play11:08

excellent an internal consistency of 0.1

play11:11

or 0.3 is not so good and keep in mind

play11:15

why we care about all of this it's

play11:18

because we want to be able to make

play11:19

accurate predictions about populations

play11:22

accurate predictions about the world we

play11:25

want to make good estimations and in

play11:27

order to estimate things well to make

play11:29

good guesses about populations we need

play11:32

to be reliable and again as we'll see in

play11:34

the future we need to be valid in how we

play11:36

measure things increasing reliability

play11:39

decreases error and more closely aligns

play11:42

our estimates with the truth

Rate This

5.0 / 5 (0 votes)

Связанные теги
ReliabilityStatisticsResearchMeasurementTest RetestParallel FormsInterraterInternal ConsistencyConsistencyAccuracy
Вам нужно краткое изложение на английском?