4. Cohort studies

Cochrane Mental Health
29 Mar 201918:20

Summary

TLDRThis video module focuses on critical appraisal of cohort studies, emphasizing their importance in healthcare research. It explains why observational studies are necessary when RCTs are not feasible or ethical. The module introduces the concept of risk ratio, illustrates its calculation with an example, and discusses how to interpret it. It also guides viewers on how to appraise cohort studies using a checklist, applying it to a study on lithium treatment and dementia risk. The video concludes with a quiz for viewers to test their understanding.

Takeaways

  • 🔍 This video module focuses on the critical appraisal of cohort studies using the CASP approach.
  • 🤔 Observational studies are necessary when RCTs are not feasible, unethical, or impractical for rare outcomes.
  • 👥 Cohort studies involve identifying participants without an outcome of interest, classifying them by exposure status, and following them over time.
  • 🚬 An example given is a cohort study on smoking and lung cancer, where researchers follow participants to see lung cancer development.
  • 📊 The risk ratio is a common measure used in cohort studies to express the difference in risk between exposed and unexposed groups.
  • 🔢 A risk ratio greater than one indicates an increased risk of disease due to exposure, while less than one suggests a protective effect.
  • 📚 The CASP checklist is introduced for critically appraising cohort studies, focusing on validity, results' trustworthiness, and relevance.
  • 📝 The study by Gerhardt et al. (2015) on lithium treatment and dementia risk is used as an example to apply the CASP checklist.
  • 🧐 Selection bias, measurement bias, and confounding are key issues to consider when critically appraising cohort studies.
  • 📈 Statistical models can adjust for confounders to provide a clearer understanding of the relationship between exposure and outcome.
  • ⏱ The duration of the follow-up period in cohort studies is crucial to allow the outcome to manifest and to maintain a representative sample.

Q & A

  • What is the main focus of the fourth video in the critical appraisal series?

    -The main focus of the fourth video is the critical appraisal of cohort studies using the CASP approach.

  • Why are observational studies necessary in healthcare research?

    -Observational studies are necessary because sometimes it's not possible or ethical to conduct randomized controlled trials (RCTs), especially when studying rare outcomes or potentially harmful interventions.

  • What is the difference between experimental studies and observational studies in terms of researcher involvement?

    -In experimental studies, researchers manipulate exposures to observe outcomes, whereas in observational studies, they do not manipulate exposures but simply observe what occurs.

  • What is a cohort study and how does it relate to risk factors and outcomes?

    -A cohort study is a type of observational study where researchers identify participants without the outcome of interest, classify them by exposure status, and follow them over time to observe the development of the outcome.

  • How is the risk ratio calculated in cohort studies?

    -The risk ratio is calculated by dividing the risk of developing the outcome in the exposed group by the risk of developing the outcome in the unexposed group.

  • What does a risk ratio greater than one indicate in a cohort study?

    -A risk ratio greater than one indicates that the exposure increases the risk of disease.

  • What is the purpose of the CASP checklist for critically appraising cohort studies?

    -The CASP checklist helps assess the validity, trustworthiness of results, and value of relevance in cohort studies.

  • What is the importance of considering confounding factors in cohort studies?

    -Confounding factors can distort the relationship between exposure and outcome, either hiding a true relationship or creating a false one. Statistically adjusting for confounders can help estimate their impact.

  • How does the length of the follow-up period in a cohort study affect the results?

    -The follow-up period should be long enough for the outcome to manifest. Insufficient follow-up can lead to incorrect measurements of the exposure-outcome relationship.

  • What does a hazard ratio represent in cohort studies and how does it differ from a risk ratio?

    -A hazard ratio represents the rate at which an event occurs over time, taking into account the duration of exposure. Unlike the risk ratio, it accounts for the timing of events.

  • How can the precision of results in cohort studies be assessed?

    -The precision of results can be assessed by examining the confidence intervals. Narrower intervals indicate higher precision.

  • What is the role of the CASP checklist in evaluating the believability of results in cohort studies?

    -The CASP checklist helps evaluate the believability of results by considering factors such as bias, confounding, and the wider body of research.

Outlines

00:00

🔎 Introduction to Cohort Studies

This paragraph introduces the critical appraisal of cohort studies within the context of healthcare research. It explains why observational studies are necessary when randomized controlled trials (RCTs) are not feasible or ethical. The paragraph outlines the purpose of cohort studies, which is to observe risk factors, exposures, and outcomes without manipulating exposures. It also introduces the concept of risk ratio as a measure to quantify risk in cohort studies and provides an example of a cohort study investigating the link between smoking and lung cancer. The risk ratio is calculated by dividing the incidence of disease in the exposed group by the incidence in the unexposed group, indicating the strength of the association between exposure and outcome.

05:01

📊 Understanding Risk Ratio and Cohort Study Appraisal

This section delves into how to calculate and interpret the risk ratio in cohort studies. It uses a fictional dataset to demonstrate the calculation process, emphasizing that a risk ratio greater than one indicates an increased risk of disease due to exposure, while a ratio less than one suggests a protective effect. The paragraph also discusses the importance of critically appraising cohort studies using a checklist to assess their validity, reliability, and relevance. An example of a cohort study by Gerhardt et al. (2015) on lithium treatment and dementia risk is presented to illustrate how the checklist is applied in practice.

10:01

🧐 Critical Appraisal Checklist Application

The paragraph discusses the application of the critical appraisal checklist to a specific cohort study. It covers various aspects such as selection bias, measurement bias, confounding, and the duration of the study's follow-up period. The study's exposure and outcome measurements are examined for reliability, and potential confounders are identified and discussed. The importance of a long enough follow-up period to observe the outcome and the impact of dropout rates on the study's validity are highlighted. The paragraph also touches on the use of statistical models to adjust for confounding factors and the interpretation of the hazard ratio, which is similar to the risk ratio but accounts for the time at which events occur.

15:02

📉 Hazard Ratio and Study Results Precision

This final paragraph focuses on the precision of study results as represented by confidence intervals and the believability of these results. It explains how confidence intervals quantify the range within which the true value is likely to lie, with narrower intervals indicating higher precision. The paragraph also discusses factors affecting the believability of results, such as bias, confounding, and the biological plausibility of the findings. It concludes by mentioning the upcoming module on case-control studies and encourages viewers to test their knowledge through an online quiz. The video series is credited to the Cochrane Common Mental Disorders Group at the University of York, with support from various NHS trusts and the Economic and Social Research Council.

Mindmap

Keywords

💡Cohort Studies

Cohort studies are a type of observational study where researchers select a group of participants who have not yet developed the outcome of interest and follow them over time to observe the development of outcomes related to exposure status. In the context of the video, cohort studies are highlighted as a crucial research design in healthcare, particularly when randomized controlled trials are not feasible or ethical. The video discusses how to critically appraise cohort studies using the CASP checklist, emphasizing their importance in understanding risk factors for diseases.

💡Critical Appraisal

Critical appraisal is the process of systematically evaluating research evidence for its validity, results' trustworthiness, and relevance to a specific context. The video focuses on teaching viewers how to critically appraise cohort studies using the CASP approach, which involves assessing the study's methodology, potential biases, and the strength of the association between exposure and outcome.

💡Risk Ratio

The risk ratio is a statistical measure used in cohort studies to express the ratio of the incidence of disease in the exposed group to the incidence in the unexposed group. It quantifies the strength of the association between an exposure and an outcome. In the video, the risk ratio is used to illustrate how smoking is associated with a significantly higher risk of lung cancer, with smokers being 17 times more likely to develop lung cancer than non-smokers.

💡Observational Studies

Observational studies are a type of research where researchers observe and analyze natural occurrences without manipulating any variables. These studies are contrasted with experimental studies in the video, where researchers actively intervene. Observational studies are emphasized for their importance in healthcare research when trials are not possible or ethical, and they are used to study rare outcomes or when testing potentially harmful agents.

💡Selection Bias

Selection bias refers to a systematic error in research studies that results from the way participants are chosen, which can lead to incorrect measurements of the relationship between exposure and outcome. The video discusses selection bias in the context of how participants are recruited for cohort studies, stressing the importance of ensuring that the sample is representative of the population to avoid biased results.

💡Measurement Bias

Measurement bias occurs when there is bias in the way exposures and outcomes are measured, which can lead to incorrect estimates of the relationship between them. The video explains measurement bias in relation to how exposure to lithium was assessed through health administrative data, suggesting that this method is more reliable than self-report, thus reducing the likelihood of measurement bias.

💡Confounding

A confounder is a factor that is independently associated with both the exposure and the outcome, and can either mask a true relationship or create a false one. The video discusses confounding in the context of adjusting for factors like age, gender, and ethnicity in statistical models to account for their potential influence on the relationship between lithium treatment and dementia risk.

💡Follow-up Period

The follow-up period in a cohort study refers to the duration of time during which participants are monitored to observe the development of the outcome. The video emphasizes the importance of having a sufficiently long follow-up period to allow the outcome to manifest and to minimize dropout rates, which could introduce bias. In the example study discussed, the maximum follow-up time was three years.

💡Hazard Ratio

The hazard ratio is a measure used in cohort studies that accounts for the rate at which an event occurs over a specific time period. It is similar to the risk ratio but adjusts for the timing of events. The video uses the hazard ratio to discuss the protective effect of lithium exposure on dementia risk, with a hazard ratio less than one indicating a reduced risk.

💡Confidence Intervals

Confidence intervals provide a range within which the true value of a population parameter is likely to fall, based on sample data. They indicate the precision of the results, with narrower intervals suggesting higher precision. The video discusses how confidence intervals can be used to assess the believability of results, taking into account factors like potential biases and the sample size.

💡Biological Plausibility

Biological plausibility refers to the extent to which a research finding is consistent with current biological understanding and the laws of nature. The video mentions considering biological plausibility when assessing the believability of results, ensuring that the relationship between exposure and outcome is not only statistically significant but also makes sense in the context of existing knowledge.

Highlights

Focus on critical appraisal of cohort studies using the CASP approach.

Why observational studies are necessary despite the strength of RCTs.

Definition and importance of cohort studies in healthcare research.

Explanation of risk ratio and its calculation in cohort studies.

Example of calculating relative risk with fictional data.

Cohort studies as the strongest design among observational studies.

Description of how cohort studies express risk differences.

Importance of critically appraising cohort studies for quality assessment.

Introduction to the CASP checklist for cohort studies.

Application of the checklist to a sample cohort study on lithium treatment and dementia risk.

Consideration of a clearly focused question in cohort studies.

Assessment of selection bias in cohort recruitment.

Measurement bias considerations in exposure and outcome assessment.

Confounding factors and their impact on study results.

Importance of adequate follow-up time in cohort studies.

Use of hazard ratio to express the association between exposure and outcome.

Precision of results represented by confidence intervals.

Assessing the believability of results in cohort studies.

Consideration of the potential local applicability of study results.

Introduction to the next module on case-control studies.

Acknowledgment of the development team and supporting organizations.

Transcripts

play00:00

welcome to the fourth video in this

play00:03

series of critical appraisal modules in

play00:06

this module we will be focusing on the

play00:09

critical appraisal of cohort studies

play00:11

using the critical appraisal skills

play00:13

program our cusp approach in the

play00:18

previous module we spoke to you about

play00:20

our cities and their importance in

play00:23

healthcare particularly in terms have

play00:26

been able to tribute causality you may

play00:29

ask why do we need observational studies

play00:33

if the RCT design is so strong in terms

play00:37

of attributing and causality but

play00:40

sometimes it isn't possible to test some

play00:44

hypotheses and trials it's also

play00:47

sometimes unethical to undertake a trial

play00:50

of an agent we believe could be harmful

play00:53

and it is not feasible to study some

play00:57

very rare outcomes and trials for this

play01:00

reason observational studies do have an

play01:04

important place in healthcare generally

play01:08

in observational studies in health

play01:10

researchers are interested in risk

play01:13

factors our exposures and outcomes often

play01:17

diseases

play01:19

unlike in experimental studies

play01:22

researchers do not manipulate exposures

play01:25

and observational studies they simply

play01:28

observe what is occurring and then they

play01:31

can calculate measures to quantify the

play01:34

extent of the risk for the learning

play01:38

outcomes we will introduce you to cohort

play01:41

studies and describe their purpose and

play01:44

value in the context of healthcare

play01:45

research and show you how we can

play01:48

critically appraise one using the class

play01:50

checklist we will also talk about the

play01:55

risk ratio how to calculate these and

play01:58

how to interpret them in cohort studies

play02:01

finally there will be a link to a short

play02:03

quiz at the end of this video which will

play02:07

give you the opportunity to test your

play02:09

knowledge and concepts we will have

play02:11

discussed using multiple

play02:13

questions and answers a cohort study is

play02:19

the strongest research design of the

play02:22

observational studies it generally

play02:25

involves the researcher identifying

play02:27

research participants who do not have

play02:30

the outcome of interest the researcher

play02:33

then classifies participants according

play02:36

to their exposure status and follows the

play02:38

participants over time to see whether or

play02:42

not they develop the outcome in question

play02:45

to take a simple example

play02:48

if a researcher is interested in whether

play02:52

or not smoking causes lung cancer the

play02:55

research you would identify a group of

play02:57

people you do not have lung cancer and

play03:00

then classify them according to whether

play03:03

or not they are smokers the researcher

play03:06

would then follow up the participants

play03:08

over time to compare rates of lung

play03:12

cancer between the groups in the cohort

play03:15

according to their smoking status so how

play03:20

do you express the different rate in

play03:22

risk between exposed and unexposed

play03:25

members of the cohort the most common

play03:29

method of doing this is the risk ratio

play03:32

the risk ratio is the ratio of the

play03:36

incidence of disease in the exposed

play03:39

group to the incidence of disease in the

play03:42

unexposed group incidence refers to new

play03:46

cases of a disease relative risk can

play03:51

quantify the strength of an association

play03:53

between exposures and outcomes if the

play03:57

relative risk is greater than one it

play04:01

means that the exposure increases the

play04:03

risk of disease the higher the number

play04:07

the greater the risk if the relative

play04:10

risk is less than one it means that the

play04:14

exposure decreases the risk of disease

play04:17

the lower the number the more the factor

play04:21

is protective if a risk ratio is exactly

play04:26

then it means there is no difference in

play04:29

risk between exposed and unexposed

play04:33

individuals so let's take an example in

play04:38

which we'll calculate the relative risk

play04:40

together

play04:41

we'll look at some fictional data

play04:45

imagine that we're still thinking about

play04:47

the relationship between smoking and

play04:50

lung cancer we might have conducted a

play04:53

cohort study and obtained the following

play04:56

results we can calculate the risk ratio

play05:01

by first calculating the risk of

play05:04

developing lung cancer in the exposed

play05:07

group then by calculating the risk of

play05:10

developing lung cancer in the unexposed

play05:13

group and then by dividing the risk in

play05:16

the exposed group to the risk in the

play05:20

unexposed group

play05:22

I've given key sections of the table the

play05:25

letters a b c and d as this is a common

play05:29

convention to calculate the risk of

play05:33

developing lung cancer in the exposed

play05:36

group it's a divided by a plus b which

play05:41

gives not point 8 5 to calculate the

play05:46

risk of developing lung cancer in the

play05:49

unexposed group it's C divided by C plus

play05:54

T which gives not point naught 5 to

play05:59

express the risk of developing lung

play06:02

cancer in the exposed to the unexposed

play06:05

group it's not point 8 5 divided by

play06:09

naught point naught 5 which gives 17 so

play06:14

that means that people who smoke are 17

play06:18

times more likely to develop lung cancer

play06:21

than non-smokers according to this

play06:24

fictional data cohort studies are an

play06:29

extremely useful study design for

play06:31

quantifying the strength of association

play06:33

between an exposure and an outcome

play06:37

however like on research that

play06:40

and healthcare their quality can vary so

play06:43

it is important that readers of cohort

play06:45

studies critically appraise the policy

play06:48

of the research surely the caste program

play06:51

has produced a checklist for critically

play06:53

appraising cohort studies which you can

play06:56

access by following the link below this

play06:58

video let's have a look at this

play07:01

checklist and then we'll apply its use

play07:03

to a sample cohort study we can see that

play07:08

because cohort studies checklist again

play07:11

separates the three key principles of

play07:14

critical appraisal of validity

play07:17

trustworthiness of results and value of

play07:21

relevance into three sections a B and C

play07:25

respectively let's see how they can be

play07:28

addressed with an example the study will

play07:33

use to work through this checklist is by

play07:35

Gerhardt's at al 2015 on lithium

play07:40

treatment and risk for dementia in

play07:43

adults with bipolar disorder

play07:46

population-based cohort study which you

play07:49

can also access by selecting the link

play07:52

below this video the first question in

play07:57

the checklist is to consider whether the

play08:00

cohort study examined a clearly focused

play08:03

question the answer for this study would

play08:06

appear to be yes as the study considered

play08:09

the association between lithium and

play08:13

dementia risk the research question is

play08:17

contextualized against the biological

play08:19

background of lithium inhibiting

play08:22

glycogen synthase kinase 3 and enzyme

play08:26

implicated in the etiology of dementia

play08:31

the second question looks at whether the

play08:34

cohort was recruited in an acceptable

play08:37

way this question is mainly about

play08:40

selection bias bias is a systematic

play08:44

error in a research study which results

play08:47

in incorrect measurement of the

play08:49

relationship between exposure our

play08:52

intervention

play08:53

and outcome selection bias refers to

play08:59

bias in terms of the way participants

play09:01

were selected as research participants

play09:04

ie when the research participants truly

play09:08

representative of the research

play09:10

population are are the participants in

play09:13

some way and typical this study's

play09:17

participants of people aged over 50 with

play09:21

a diagnosis of bipolar disorder from 8

play09:24

large US states and Medicaid insured

play09:27

u.s. population Medicaid is a social

play09:32

health care program for Americans with

play09:34

disabilities are lower incomes

play09:38

the third question considers a different

play09:42

type of bias namely measurement bias

play09:45

this refers to bias in terms of the way

play09:49

exposure and our outcome a measured

play09:52

which could lead to an incorrect

play09:54

estimate of the relationship between

play09:57

exposure and outcome it is important to

play10:01

consider factors such as whether or not

play10:04

exposures were assessed via self-report

play10:06

as this may be affected by social

play10:10

desirability bias are the fal ability of

play10:13

memory the exposure in this study was

play10:18

lithium use and it was assessed by a

play10:20

health administrative data so it is more

play10:23

likely that the measurement of the

play10:25

exposure and outcome are more reliable

play10:28

than self-report measurements this

play10:33

question relates to a session for

play10:36

possible measurement bias in measuring

play10:39

outcome which in this case was

play10:42

development of dementia things to

play10:45

consider in this criteria relate to

play10:48

whether or not the outcome was measured

play10:50

subjectively or objectively with

play10:53

objective measures of outcome been

play10:56

generally more reliable and to consider

play10:59

whether or not Assessors were blind to

play11:02

exposure status this means whether or

play11:05

not to assess

play11:06

knew whether or not a person had or have

play11:09

not been exposed as if they know this it

play11:13

might affect their assessment of outcome

play11:16

particularly in more subjective outcome

play11:19

measurements sometimes however the fact

play11:23

of whether or not Assessors are blind is

play11:25

not so essential if the outcome is

play11:27

clear-cut for example this studies

play11:30

outcome is more of a factual objective

play11:34

outcome less likely to be affected by

play11:37

Assessors knowledge of exposure status

play11:39

and the use of health administrative

play11:41

data lessens the likelihood that the

play11:44

results of the study have been

play11:46

compromised by measurement bias in

play11:48

assessing the outcome this question

play11:53

relates confounding a confounder is a

play11:57

factor which is independently associated

play12:00

with an exposure and an outcome and

play12:03

which can either hide a true

play12:05

relationship between an exposure and

play12:07

outcome or make it seem like there is a

play12:10

relationship between exposure and

play12:12

outcome when in facts there is no

play12:16

relationship possible confounders can

play12:19

often relate to age gender or health

play12:23

related behaviors such as dietary

play12:25

factors or whether or not people

play12:27

exercise it is possible to estimate the

play12:32

impact which confounders may have on a

play12:35

research study by statistically

play12:37

adjusting for them when analyzing the

play12:39

relationship between exposure and

play12:41

outcome in this study the Astor's

play12:45

present an adjusted statistical model a

play12:48

model which adjusted for gender age and

play12:51

ethnicity all of which are common

play12:53

confounders and a statistical model

play12:56

which suggested for age gender and

play13:00

ethnicity as well as other factors which

play13:03

are potential confounders the authors do

play13:06

note that Nassim treated patients were

play13:09

found to have a lower risk of developing

play13:11

dementia in their study but also that

play13:14

lycium treated patients a lower rates of

play13:17

baseline cerebrovascular

play13:19

disease and diabetes most of which are

play13:22

actually risk factors for dementia and

play13:25

therefore factors which could be

play13:27

confounders this question relates the

play13:33

time spent and following up for the

play13:34

cohort cohort studies are generally

play13:38

undertaken over several years and it is

play13:41

important that the study follow-up

play13:43

period is long enough for the outcome to

play13:45

manifest itself it's also important that

play13:49

the study tries to collect data from as

play13:51

many people who started in the cohort as

play13:54

possible and not to allow people to drop

play13:57

out as the people who do drop out may

play14:00

not be typical of the ones remain in the

play14:02

study and this again could lead to an

play14:05

incorrect measurement of the

play14:07

relationship between exposure and

play14:08

outcome the maximum follow-up time

play14:11

period in this study was three years and

play14:14

judgment is required as to whether or

play14:17

not this outcome period is long enough

play14:20

to allow development of the outcome

play14:22

versus the outcome will be occurring in

play14:24

this study the authors found that 301 to

play14:29

365 days of lithium exposure was

play14:32

associated with reduced dementia risk

play14:34

but that no association was found for

play14:38

shorter duration of exposure to lithium

play14:40

the measurement used to express the

play14:43

association between lissome exposure and

play14:46

dementia risk is the hazard ratio which

play14:49

is similar to the risk ratio we looked

play14:51

at earlier but the hazard ratio accounts

play14:54

for the rate or time period at which

play14:57

event to happen and instead of just

play14:59

looking at whether or not an event

play15:01

happened the hazard ratio for 301 to 365

play15:06

days of lithium exposure was naught

play15:09

point seven seven meaning that lithium

play15:11

exposure is protective because the

play15:14

hazard ratio is less than one like risk

play15:18

ratios if a hazard ratio is under 1 then

play15:22

this means that an exposure is

play15:24

protective whereas if a hazard ratio is

play15:28

higher than one then this means that an

play15:30

exposure raises the

play15:32

of an outcome occurring the precision of

play15:37

results are represented by confidence

play15:40

intervals confidence intervals relate to

play15:43

the range in which the true value lies a

play15:46

research study generally involves a

play15:49

sample rather than everyone in the

play15:51

research population and even in a

play15:54

robustly conducted study there will

play15:57

always be some error in the sample

play15:59

compared to the population a confidence

play16:03

interval quantifies this by

play16:05

acknowledging that while the sample

play16:07

produced a certain value the true value

play16:10

in the population is likely to be within

play16:13

a certain range a narrower confidence

play16:16

interval indicates higher precision of

play16:19

results so a confidence interval of

play16:22

three to far around the risk ratio of

play16:25

three point five would be more precise

play16:27

than a confidence interval of t27 around

play16:31

the same risk ratio three point five the

play16:35

believability of results can be assessed

play16:38

by considering the factors we have

play16:40

previously discussed such as confirmed

play16:43

in our bias it's also important to

play16:47

consider whether the results may have

play16:50

been affected by chance assessing the

play16:53

believability of results also requires

play16:56

judgment and consideration of factors

play16:59

such as the biological plausibility of

play17:02

the relationship between exposure and

play17:04

outcome and the contextualization of the

play17:08

cohort study in the wider body of

play17:10

research in the field questions 10 11

play17:14

and 12 of the cusp checklist follow on

play17:17

from this theme and require judgment to

play17:20

consider the potential local

play17:22

applicability of the results the fifth

play17:26

module in this series will look at case

play17:29

control studies and we will be following

play17:31

a similar format as we used in this

play17:34

video to appraise an example of a recent

play17:37

study thank you for listening these

play17:42

training videos have been developed by

play17:44

the Cochran common mental

play17:46

Dada's group at the University of York

play17:49

with support from TSS can wear valleys

play17:52

NHS foundation trust Northumberland Tyne

play17:56

and Wear NHS Foundation Trust and the

play17:59

Economic and Social Research Council if

play18:03

you would like to test your knowledge on

play18:05

the topics introduced in this module

play18:07

please follow the link below which will

play18:10

take you to a short online quiz

Rate This

5.0 / 5 (0 votes)

الوسوم ذات الصلة
Healthcare ResearchCohort StudiesCritical AppraisalObservational StudiesRisk FactorsEthical TrialsDementia RiskLithium TreatmentBipolar DisorderConfounding FactorsStatistical Models
هل تحتاج إلى تلخيص باللغة الإنجليزية؟