Annie Murphy Paul: What we learn before we're born

TED
29 Nov 201116:47

Summary

TLDRThis talk explores the surprising concept of fetal learning, revealing that significant learning occurs in the womb. Babies can recognize their mother's voice and even the language's melodic contours from birth. They also develop taste preferences from the flavors in amniotic fluid, influenced by their mother's diet. The lecture delves into how the in utero environment shapes a child's future health, suggesting that the fetus adapts its physiology to anticipate post-birth conditions based on maternal cues, with profound implications for lifelong well-being.

Takeaways

  • πŸ“š Learning begins before birth, with the most critical period of learning happening in the womb.
  • πŸ‘Ά Fetuses learn to recognize their mother's voice as early as the fourth month of gestation, preferring it over others after birth.
  • 🎡 Babies can also identify familiar sounds from the womb, such as a theme song from a soap opera their mothers watched regularly.
  • πŸ—£οΈ Newborns cry in the accent of their mother's native language, indicating in-utero exposure to linguistic patterns.
  • 🍼 Fetuses develop a preference for tastes they are exposed to in the womb, such as carrot juice, which influences their food preferences post-birth.
  • 🌿 The flavors and spices of a mother's diet can introduce the fetus to the cuisine of their culture, even before they are born.
  • 🧬 Fetal learning is not just about sensory experiences; it also involves preparing for the world's conditions, such as abundance or scarcity.
  • 🍎 The diet and stress levels of a pregnant woman provide clues to the environment, influencing the fetus's physiological development.
  • ❄️ The 'Hunger Winter' of 1944-1945 in the Netherlands showed that prenatal experiences, such as malnutrition, can have long-term health effects.
  • πŸŒ† The story of the Dutch 'Hunger Winter' illustrates how a fetus's expectations of the world, formed in the womb, can lead to health issues if the postnatal environment differs.
  • 🀰 The experiences and emotions of a pregnant woman, such as those who experienced the 9/11 attacks, can leave a biological mark on the fetus, potentially affecting their future health.

Q & A

  • What is the main topic of the speaker's presentation?

    -The main topic is the concept of learning beginning before birth, focusing on the importance of fetal learning and its impact on an individual's health and well-being throughout life.

  • What is the 'Zero-to-Three movement' mentioned in the script?

    -The 'Zero-to-Three movement' is an assertion that the most critical years for learning are the earliest ones, emphasizing the importance of the first three years of life in a child's development.

  • What is the 'fetal origins' field of study?

    -The 'fetal origins' field is a scientific discipline that emerged about two decades ago, based on the theory that a person's health and well-being throughout life is significantly influenced by the nine months spent in the womb.

  • How do fetuses learn the sound of their mother's voice?

    -Fetuses learn the sound of their mother's voice because it reverberates through her body and reaches the fetus more readily than external sounds, which are muffled by the abdominal tissue and amniotic fluid.

  • What experiment showed that newborn babies recognize a passage from 'The Cat in the Hat' read aloud by their mothers during pregnancy?

    -Researchers used a method where babies would hear a recording of their mother's voice or a stranger's voice through headphones when they sucked on different rubber nipples, and they found that babies recognized the passage from 'The Cat in the Hat' when it was read to them after birth.

  • How do babies demonstrate their preference for their mother's voice over a stranger's?

    -Babies demonstrate their preference by choosing to suck on the nipple that plays a recording of their mother's voice and by slowing down their sucking when they are interested in what they hear, which indicates recognition.

  • What does the script suggest about babies crying in the accent of their mother's native language?

    -The script suggests that from birth, babies cry in the accent of their mother's native language, indicating that they have learned about the language's melodic contours while in the womb.

  • How do fetuses learn about tastes and smells?

    -Fetuses learn about tastes and smells through the flavors of the food the pregnant woman eats, which find their way into the amniotic fluid and are continuously swallowed by the fetus.

  • What experiment showed that babies preferred the taste of carrot juice if their mothers drank it during pregnancy?

    -In an experiment, pregnant women were asked to drink a lot of carrot juice during their third trimester, and later, their infants were offered cereal mixed with carrot juice. The babies showed a preference for the carrot-flavored cereal, indicating they remembered and preferred this taste.

  • What is the significance of the 'Hunger Winter' of 1944 in the context of fetal learning?

    -The 'Hunger Winter' of 1944 in the Netherlands is significant because it provided evidence that the prenatal experience of malnutrition can have long-term health effects, such as increased rates of obesity, diabetes, and heart disease later in life.

  • How did the script address the potential misconception about fetal learning and the influence of external stimuli like Mozart's music?

    -The script clarifies that fetal learning is not about external enrichment like playing Mozart's music to the fetus but is a more visceral process where the fetus incorporates the mother's experiences, such as diet, stress, and emotions, into its own development.

  • What does the script suggest about the potential adaptive value of prenatal transmission of PTSD risk?

    -The script suggests that the prenatal transmission of PTSD risk might be an adaptive response, preparing the fetus for a potentially dangerous environment by increasing hyper-awareness and quick-trigger responses to danger.

  • What is the overall message the speaker wants to convey about the importance of fetal learning?

    -The overall message is that learning begins much earlier than previously thought, and what fetuses learn during their time in the womb is crucial for their future health and well-being, emphasizing the importance of focusing on this period for promoting the health of the next generation.

Outlines

00:00

πŸ‘Ά Prenatal Learning and Its Impacts

This paragraph introduces the concept of learning beginning before birth, challenging the traditional view that learning starts at preschool or kindergarten. The speaker, a science reporter and mother, discusses the emerging field of 'fetal origins,' which explores how our health and well-being are influenced by the time spent in the womb. The paragraph highlights that babies learn their mother's voice in the womb and prefer it after birth, as demonstrated through experiments using babies' sucking behavior. It also touches on how babies can recognize stories or songs they were exposed to in the womb, indicating that learning occurs even before birth.

05:02

🍼 Fetal Learning of Language and Taste

The second paragraph delves into the evolutionary benefits of fetal learning, suggesting that babies are born with a preference for their mother's voice and the language she speaks, which aids in their survival and language acquisition. It also discusses how fetuses develop a sense of taste and smell, with experiments showing that they prefer flavors they were exposed to in the womb. This learning is not just about individual tastes but also about cultural flavors, as the food a pregnant woman eats influences the baby's future food preferences, effectively teaching them about their culture's cuisine before birth.

10:06

🌱 The Long-Term Effects of Prenatal Environment

This paragraph examines the profound and lasting effects of the prenatal environment on a child's health and development. It recounts the 'Hunger Winter' of 1944 in the Netherlands, during which pregnant women experienced severe malnutrition, leading to long-term health issues for their children, including obesity, diabetes, and heart disease. The discussion suggests that the fetus adapts its physiology based on the prenatal environment, preparing for the world it anticipates post-birth. However, when the anticipated and actual environments mismatch, as with the Dutch 'Hunger Winter' babies, it can lead to health complications.

15:07

🚨 Prenatal Stress and Its Legacy

The final paragraph addresses the impact of prenatal stress, using the example of pregnant women who were in the vicinity of the World Trade Center during the 9/11 attacks. It reveals that these women's children showed a heightened susceptibility to post-traumatic stress syndrome (PTSD), suggesting a biological transmission of stress vulnerability. The speaker speculates that this could be an adaptive response, preparing the child for a potentially dangerous world. The paragraph concludes by emphasizing the importance of understanding fetal learning for promoting the health and well-being of future generations, without placing blame on women for pregnancy experiences.

Mindmap

Keywords

πŸ’‘Learning

Learning is the process of acquiring new knowledge, skills, or understanding through experience or study. In the context of the video, learning is not limited to formal education but begins at birth and even before, with the fetus learning in the womb. The video script emphasizes that learning is a fundamental activity that starts much earlier than we traditionally thought, with the fetus being shaped by the world even before birth.

πŸ’‘Fetal Origins

Fetal Origins refers to a scientific discipline that emerged about two decades ago, based on the theory that our health and well-being throughout our lives are crucially affected by the nine months we spend in the womb. The video discusses this concept extensively, highlighting how the environment and experiences of a pregnant woman can have lasting impacts on the baby's future health and development.

πŸ’‘Gestation

Gestation is the period of development inside the womb before birth. The video script mentions the fourth month of gestation as a time when fetuses start to hear sounds, indicating that learning can occur during this period. Gestation is a critical time for the fetus to learn and adapt to the world it will be born into.

πŸ’‘Voice Recognition

Voice Recognition in the video refers to the ability of a fetus to recognize and prefer the sound of its mother's voice over others. The script describes experiments where newborns show a preference for their mother's voice, demonstrating that learning about the mother's voice begins in utero and has implications for the baby's attachment and language development.

πŸ’‘Amniotic Fluid

Amniotic Fluid is the protective liquid that surrounds a fetus in the womb. The script mentions that sounds from the outside world have to travel through the mother's abdominal tissue and the amniotic fluid to reach the fetus. This fluid plays a role in how sounds are transmitted and perceived by the fetus, affecting its learning about the outside world.

πŸ’‘Taste Buds

Taste Buds are the sensory structures on the tongue that enable the sense of taste. The video script explains that by seven months of gestation, a fetus' taste buds are fully developed, and it can taste flavors from the food the mother eats, which are found in the amniotic fluid. This ability contributes to the baby's later food preferences and cultural dietary habits.

πŸ’‘Olfactory Receptors

Olfactory Receptors are specialized cells that allow for the sense of smell. The script states that these receptors are functioning by the seventh month of gestation, enabling the fetus to smell and potentially learn about the scents of the world around it, which can influence its preferences and behaviors after birth.

πŸ’‘Cultural Cuisine

Cultural Cuisine refers to the characteristic flavors and spices of a particular culture's food. The video script suggests that fetuses are introduced to these flavors even before birth, learning about the culture they will join through the foods their mothers consume, which can influence their dietary preferences later in life.

πŸ’‘PTSD

PTSD stands for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder, a mental health condition triggered by experiencing or witnessing a terrifying event. The script discusses how pregnant women exposed to the 9/11 attacks and who developed PTSD may have passed on a vulnerability to PTSD to their unborn children, illustrating the profound impact of maternal stress on fetal development.

πŸ’‘Adaptation

Adaptation in the context of the video refers to the process by which the fetus adjusts its physiology and metabolism in anticipation of the environment it will encounter after birth. The script explains that the fetus uses information from the mother's diet and stress levels to prepare for the world, which can have long-term implications for its health and well-being.

πŸ’‘Malnutrition

Malnutrition is a state where the body isn't getting enough nutrients. The video script uses the example of the Dutch 'Hunger Winter' to illustrate how malnutrition during pregnancy can have immediate and long-term effects on the fetus, including increased rates of obesity, diabetes, and heart disease later in life.

Highlights

Learning begins before birth, influenced by the environment in the womb.

Fetal origins is a scientific field studying the impact of the womb on lifelong health.

Fetuses learn to recognize their mother's voice as early as the fourth month of gestation.

Newborns prefer the sound of their mother's voice over a stranger's.

Babies can recognize passages read to them in the womb, demonstrated through sucking behavior.

Fetuses can also recognize the theme song of a soap opera watched by the mother during pregnancy.

Babies cry in the accent of their mother's native language from birth.

Fetal learning of language may aid in survival and bonding with the mother.

Fetuses develop taste and smell senses, influenced by the flavors in the amniotic fluid.

Infants show preference for flavors they were exposed to in the womb, such as carrot juice.

Fetuses are introduced to cultural flavors and spices, preparing them for their cultural environment.

Fetal learning is more than just sounds; it includes tastes, smells, and even stress responses.

The womb environment, including diet and stress, shapes the fetus's future health and adaptability.

The Dutch 'Hunger Winter' study shows long-term health effects of prenatal malnutrition.

Prenatal experiences, like starvation, can change a fetus's physiology to prepare for the expected environment.

Mismatch between prenatal expectations and postnatal reality can lead to health issues, such as obesity and diabetes.

The 9/11 study shows that maternal stress can affect the baby's susceptibility to PTSD.

Fetal origins research aims to promote health and well-being, not to blame women.

Learning begins much earlier than previously thought, with significant implications for early development.

Transcripts

play00:15

My subject today is learning.

play00:18

And in that spirit, I want to spring on you all a pop quiz.

play00:21

Ready?

play00:23

When does learning begin?

play00:26

Now as you ponder that question,

play00:28

maybe you're thinking about the first day of preschool

play00:30

or kindergarten,

play00:32

the first time that kids are in a classroom with a teacher.

play00:35

Or maybe you've called to mind the toddler phase

play00:38

when children are learning how to walk and talk

play00:41

and use a fork.

play00:43

Maybe you've encountered the Zero-to-Three movement,

play00:46

which asserts that the most important years for learning

play00:49

are the earliest ones.

play00:51

And so your answer to my question would be:

play00:54

Learning begins at birth.

play00:56

Well today I want to present to you

play00:58

an idea that may be surprising

play01:01

and may even seem implausible,

play01:04

but which is supported by the latest evidence

play01:06

from psychology and biology.

play01:09

And that is that some of the most important learning we ever do

play01:12

happens before we're born,

play01:14

while we're still in the womb.

play01:17

Now I'm a science reporter.

play01:19

I write books and magazine articles.

play01:21

And I'm also a mother.

play01:23

And those two roles came together for me

play01:26

in a book that I wrote called "Origins."

play01:29

"Origins" is a report from the front lines

play01:32

of an exciting new field

play01:34

called fetal origins.

play01:36

Fetal origins is a scientific discipline

play01:39

that emerged just about two decades ago,

play01:42

and it's based on the theory

play01:45

that our health and well-being throughout our lives

play01:48

is crucially affected

play01:50

by the nine months we spend in the womb.

play01:53

Now this theory was of more than just intellectual interest to me.

play01:57

I was myself pregnant

play01:59

while I was doing the research for the book.

play02:02

And one of the most fascinating insights

play02:04

I took from this work

play02:06

is that we're all learning about the world

play02:09

even before we enter it.

play02:12

When we hold our babies for the first time,

play02:14

we might imagine that they're clean slates,

play02:17

unmarked by life,

play02:19

when in fact, they've already been shaped by us

play02:22

and by the particular world we live in.

play02:26

Today I want to share with you some of the amazing things

play02:28

that scientists are discovering

play02:30

about what fetuses learn

play02:32

while they're still in their mothers' bellies.

play02:36

First of all,

play02:38

they learn the sound of their mothers' voices.

play02:41

Because sounds from the outside world

play02:44

have to travel through the mother's abdominal tissue

play02:47

and through the amniotic fluid that surrounds the fetus,

play02:51

the voices fetuses hear,

play02:53

starting around the fourth month of gestation,

play02:56

are muted and muffled.

play02:58

One researcher says

play03:00

that they probably sound a lot like the the voice of Charlie Brown's teacher

play03:03

in the old "Peanuts" cartoon.

play03:06

But the pregnant woman's own voice

play03:09

reverberates through her body,

play03:11

reaching the fetus much more readily.

play03:14

And because the fetus is with her all the time,

play03:17

it hears her voice a lot.

play03:20

Once the baby's born, it recognizes her voice

play03:23

and it prefers listening to her voice

play03:25

over anyone else's.

play03:27

How can we know this?

play03:29

Newborn babies can't do much,

play03:31

but one thing they're really good at is sucking.

play03:34

Researchers take advantage of this fact

play03:37

by rigging up two rubber nipples,

play03:40

so that if a baby sucks on one,

play03:42

it hears a recording of its mother's voice

play03:44

on a pair of headphones,

play03:46

and if it sucks on the other nipple,

play03:48

it hears a recording of a female stranger's voice.

play03:52

Babies quickly show their preference

play03:55

by choosing the first one.

play03:58

Scientists also take advantage of the fact

play04:01

that babies will slow down their sucking

play04:03

when something interests them

play04:05

and resume their fast sucking

play04:07

when they get bored.

play04:10

This is how researchers discovered

play04:12

that, after women repeatedly read aloud

play04:15

a section of Dr. Seuss' "The Cat in the Hat" while they were pregnant,

play04:19

their newborn babies recognized that passage

play04:22

when they hear it outside the womb.

play04:25

My favorite experiment of this kind

play04:28

is the one that showed that the babies

play04:30

of women who watched a certain soap opera

play04:32

every day during pregnancy

play04:35

recognized the theme song of that show

play04:38

once they were born.

play04:41

So fetuses are even learning

play04:43

about the particular language that's spoken

play04:46

in the world that they'll be born into.

play04:48

A study published last year

play04:51

found that from birth, from the moment of birth,

play04:54

babies cry in the accent

play04:56

of their mother's native language.

play04:59

French babies cry on a rising note

play05:02

while German babies end on a falling note,

play05:05

imitating the melodic contours

play05:07

of those languages.

play05:09

Now why would this kind of fetal learning

play05:11

be useful?

play05:13

It may have evolved to aid the baby's survival.

play05:16

From the moment of birth,

play05:18

the baby responds most to the voice

play05:20

of the person who is most likely to care for it --

play05:22

its mother.

play05:24

It even makes its cries

play05:26

sound like the mother's language,

play05:28

which may further endear the baby to the mother,

play05:31

and which may give the baby a head start

play05:33

in the critical task

play05:35

of learning how to understand and speak

play05:38

its native language.

play05:40

But it's not just sounds

play05:42

that fetuses are learning about in utero.

play05:44

It's also tastes and smells.

play05:47

By seven months of gestation,

play05:49

the fetus' taste buds are fully developed,

play05:51

and its olfactory receptors, which allow it to smell,

play05:54

are functioning.

play05:56

The flavors of the food a pregnant woman eats

play05:59

find their way into the amniotic fluid,

play06:01

which is continuously swallowed

play06:03

by the fetus.

play06:05

Babies seem to remember and prefer these tastes

play06:08

once they're out in the world.

play06:11

In one experiment, a group of pregnant women

play06:14

was asked to drink a lot of carrot juice

play06:16

during their third trimester of pregnancy,

play06:19

while another group of pregnant women

play06:21

drank only water.

play06:23

Six months later, the women's infants

play06:26

were offered cereal mixed with carrot juice,

play06:29

and their facial expressions were observed while they ate it.

play06:33

The offspring of the carrot juice drinking women

play06:35

ate more carrot-flavored cereal,

play06:37

and from the looks of it,

play06:39

they seemed to enjoy it more.

play06:41

A sort of French version of this experiment

play06:44

was carried out in Dijon, France

play06:46

where researchers found

play06:48

that mothers who consumed food and drink

play06:51

flavored with licorice-flavored anise during pregnancy

play06:56

showed a preference for anise

play06:58

on their first day of life,

play07:00

and again, when they were tested later,

play07:02

on their fourth day of life.

play07:04

Babies whose mothers did not eat anise during pregnancy

play07:08

showed a reaction that translated roughly as "yuck."

play07:12

What this means

play07:14

is that fetuses are effectively being taught by their mothers

play07:16

about what is safe and good to eat.

play07:19

Fetuses are also being taught

play07:21

about the particular culture that they'll be joining

play07:24

through one of culture's most powerful expressions,

play07:27

which is food.

play07:29

They're being introduced to the characteristic flavors and spices

play07:32

of their culture's cuisine

play07:34

even before birth.

play07:37

Now it turns out that fetuses are learning even bigger lessons.

play07:40

But before I get to that,

play07:42

I want to address something that you may be wondering about.

play07:46

The notion of fetal learning

play07:48

may conjure up for you attempts to enrich the fetus --

play07:51

like playing Mozart through headphones

play07:53

placed on a pregnant belly.

play07:55

But actually, the nine-month-long process

play07:58

of molding and shaping that goes on in the womb

play08:01

is a lot more visceral and consequential than that.

play08:05

Much of what a pregnant woman encounters in her daily life --

play08:09

the air she breathes,

play08:11

the food and drink she consumes,

play08:13

the chemicals she's exposed to,

play08:15

even the emotions she feels --

play08:17

are shared in some fashion with her fetus.

play08:20

They make up a mix of influences

play08:23

as individual and idiosyncratic

play08:25

as the woman herself.

play08:27

The fetus incorporates these offerings

play08:29

into its own body,

play08:31

makes them part of its flesh and blood.

play08:34

And often it does something more.

play08:36

It treats these maternal contributions

play08:39

as information,

play08:41

as what I like to call biological postcards

play08:43

from the world outside.

play08:46

So what a fetus is learning about in utero

play08:49

is not Mozart's "Magic Flute"

play08:51

but answers to questions much more critical to its survival.

play08:55

Will it be born into a world of abundance

play08:57

or scarcity?

play08:59

Will it be safe and protected,

play09:02

or will it face constant dangers and threats?

play09:05

Will it live a long, fruitful life

play09:07

or a short, harried one?

play09:10

The pregnant woman's diet and stress level in particular

play09:13

provide important clues to prevailing conditions

play09:16

like a finger lifted to the wind.

play09:19

The resulting tuning and tweaking

play09:21

of a fetus' brain and other organs

play09:24

are part of what give us humans

play09:26

our enormous flexibility,

play09:28

our ability to thrive

play09:30

in a huge variety of environments,

play09:32

from the country to the city,

play09:34

from the tundra to the desert.

play09:37

To conclude, I want to tell you two stories

play09:39

about how mothers teach their children about the world

play09:42

even before they're born.

play09:46

In the autumn of 1944,

play09:48

the darkest days of World War II,

play09:51

German troops blockaded Western Holland,

play09:54

turning away all shipments of food.

play09:57

The opening of the Nazi's siege

play09:59

was followed by one of the harshest winters in decades --

play10:02

so cold the water in the canals froze solid.

play10:06

Soon food became scarce,

play10:08

with many Dutch surviving on just 500 calories a day --

play10:12

a quarter of what they consumed before the war.

play10:15

As weeks of deprivation stretched into months,

play10:18

some resorted to eating tulip bulbs.

play10:21

By the beginning of May,

play10:23

the nation's carefully rationed food reserve

play10:25

was completely exhausted.

play10:27

The specter of mass starvation loomed.

play10:30

And then on May 5th, 1945,

play10:33

the siege came to a sudden end

play10:35

when Holland was liberated

play10:37

by the Allies.

play10:39

The "Hunger Winter," as it came to be known,

play10:42

killed some 10,000 people

play10:44

and weakened thousands more.

play10:46

But there was another population that was affected --

play10:49

the 40,000 fetuses

play10:51

in utero during the siege.

play10:54

Some of the effects of malnutrition during pregnancy

play10:56

were immediately apparent

play10:58

in higher rates of stillbirths,

play11:00

birth defects, low birth weights

play11:02

and infant mortality.

play11:04

But others wouldn't be discovered for many years.

play11:07

Decades after the "Hunger Winter,"

play11:09

researchers documented

play11:11

that people whose mothers were pregnant during the siege

play11:15

have more obesity, more diabetes

play11:17

and more heart disease in later life

play11:20

than individuals who were gestated under normal conditions.

play11:23

These individuals' prenatal experience of starvation

play11:27

seems to have changed their bodies

play11:29

in myriad ways.

play11:31

They have higher blood pressure,

play11:33

poorer cholesterol profiles

play11:35

and reduced glucose tolerance --

play11:37

a precursor of diabetes.

play11:40

Why would undernutrition in the womb

play11:42

result in disease later?

play11:44

One explanation

play11:46

is that fetuses are making the best of a bad situation.

play11:49

When food is scarce,

play11:51

they divert nutrients towards the really critical organ, the brain,

play11:54

and away from other organs

play11:56

like the heart and liver.

play11:58

This keeps the fetus alive in the short-term,

play12:01

but the bill comes due later on in life

play12:04

when those other organs, deprived early on,

play12:06

become more susceptible to disease.

play12:09

But that may not be all that's going on.

play12:12

It seems that fetuses are taking cues

play12:14

from the intrauterine environment

play12:17

and tailoring their physiology accordingly.

play12:19

They're preparing themselves

play12:21

for the kind of world they will encounter

play12:23

on the other side of the womb.

play12:25

The fetus adjusts its metabolism

play12:27

and other physiological processes

play12:30

in anticipation of the environment that awaits it.

play12:33

And the basis of the fetus' prediction

play12:36

is what its mother eats.

play12:38

The meals a pregnant woman consumes

play12:40

constitute a kind of story,

play12:42

a fairy tale of abundance

play12:44

or a grim chronicle of deprivation.

play12:47

This story imparts information

play12:50

that the fetus uses

play12:52

to organize its body and its systems --

play12:54

an adaptation to prevailing circumstances

play12:57

that facilitates its future survival.

play13:00

Faced with severely limited resources,

play13:03

a smaller-sized child with reduced energy requirements

play13:06

will, in fact, have a better chance

play13:08

of living to adulthood.

play13:10

The real trouble comes

play13:12

when pregnant women are, in a sense, unreliable narrators,

play13:15

when fetuses are led

play13:17

to expect a world of scarcity

play13:19

and are born instead into a world of plenty.

play13:22

This is what happened to the children of the Dutch "Hunger Winter."

play13:25

And their higher rates of obesity,

play13:27

diabetes and heart disease

play13:29

are the result.

play13:31

Bodies that were built to hang onto every calorie

play13:34

found themselves swimming in the superfluous calories

play13:36

of the post-war Western diet.

play13:39

The world they had learned about while in utero

play13:42

was not the same

play13:44

as the world into which they were born.

play13:47

Here's another story.

play13:49

At 8:46 a.m. on September 11th, 2001,

play13:53

there were tens of thousands of people

play13:55

in the vicinity of the World Trade Center

play13:57

in New York --

play13:59

commuters spilling off trains,

play14:01

waitresses setting tables for the morning rush,

play14:04

brokers already working the phones on Wall Street.

play14:08

1,700 of these people were pregnant women.

play14:11

When the planes struck and the towers collapsed,

play14:14

many of these women experienced the same horrors

play14:17

inflicted on other survivors of the disaster --

play14:20

the overwhelming chaos and confusion,

play14:22

the rolling clouds

play14:24

of potentially toxic dust and debris,

play14:28

the heart-pounding fear for their lives.

play14:30

About a year after 9/11,

play14:32

researchers examined a group of women

play14:35

who were pregnant

play14:37

when they were exposed to the World Trade Center attack.

play14:39

In the babies of those women

play14:41

who developed post-traumatic stress syndrome, or PTSD,

play14:44

following their ordeal,

play14:46

researchers discovered a biological marker

play14:49

of susceptibility to PTSD --

play14:51

an effect that was most pronounced

play14:54

in infants whose mothers experienced the catastrophe

play14:57

in their third trimester.

play14:59

In other words,

play15:01

the mothers with post-traumatic stress syndrome

play15:04

had passed on a vulnerability to the condition

play15:07

to their children while they were still in utero.

play15:10

Now consider this:

play15:12

post-traumatic stress syndrome

play15:14

appears to be a reaction to stress gone very wrong,

play15:17

causing its victims tremendous unnecessary suffering.

play15:21

But there's another way of thinking about PTSD.

play15:24

What looks like pathology to us

play15:27

may actually be a useful adaptation

play15:29

in some circumstances.

play15:31

In a particularly dangerous environment,

play15:34

the characteristic manifestations of PTSD --

play15:37

a hyper-awareness of one's surroundings,

play15:40

a quick-trigger response to danger --

play15:43

could save someone's life.

play15:46

The notion that the prenatal transmission of PTSD risk is adaptive

play15:50

is still speculative,

play15:52

but I find it rather poignant.

play15:55

It would mean that, even before birth,

play15:57

mothers are warning their children

play15:59

that it's a wild world out there,

play16:01

telling them, "Be careful."

play16:04

Let me be clear.

play16:06

Fetal origins research is not about blaming women

play16:09

for what happens during pregnancy.

play16:11

It's about discovering how best to promote

play16:14

the health and well-being of the next generation.

play16:17

That important effort must include a focus

play16:19

on what fetuses learn

play16:21

during the nine months they spend in the womb.

play16:24

Learning is one of life's most essential activities,

play16:27

and it begins much earlier

play16:29

than we ever imagined.

play16:31

Thank you.

play16:33

(Applause)

Rate This
β˜…
β˜…
β˜…
β˜…
β˜…

5.0 / 5 (0 votes)

Related Tags
Fetal LearningPrenatal DevelopmentInfant BehaviorMaternal InfluenceLanguage AcquisitionCultural AdaptationHealth ImplicationsNutrition ImpactEmotional TraumaSurvival StrategiesDevelopmental Biology