Would you sacrifice one person to save five? - Eleanor Nelsen

TED-Ed
12 Jan 201704:56

Summary

TLDRThe trolley problem, introduced by philosopher Philippa Foot, presents a moral dilemma where sacrificing one life to save five is debated. Utilitarianism supports the choice of maximizing well-being, yet people's instincts often oppose deliberately causing death. The script explores variations of the problem, revealing psychological factors influencing ethical judgments. It also discusses the relevance of such dilemmas in programming ethics into autonomous systems like driverless cars and military drones, highlighting the importance of defining human life value and the greater good.

Takeaways

  • ๐Ÿš‚ The Trolley Problem is an ethical dilemma that presents a choice between sacrificing one person to save five, challenging our moral decision-making.
  • ๐Ÿค” It was devised by philosopher Philippa Foot in 1967 and is popular for forcing us to consider the absence of a 'good' choice.
  • ๐Ÿ“Š A majority of respondents in surveys, around 90%, choose to flip the switch, aligning with the utilitarian principle of maximizing well-being for the most people.
  • ๐Ÿ”„ The dilemma changes when the scenario involves pushing a person to their death to save five, with only about 10% agreeing to this action, showing a difference in moral perception.
  • ๐Ÿง  The script discusses the intersection of ethics and psychology, highlighting that our moral judgments are influenced by more than just logical reasoning.
  • ๐Ÿ‘จโ€๐Ÿฆฑ Gender and mood, such as watching a comedy clip, can influence responses to the trolley problem, with men and those in a lighter mood more likely to choose the utilitarian option.
  • ๐Ÿง The script reveals that the brain's emotional response and areas involved in decision-making are activated differently in the two scenarios, with the 'bridge' version eliciting stronger emotional reactions.
  • ๐Ÿค– The trolley problem is relevant to modern technology, such as autonomous vehicles and military drones, which may need to make similar ethical decisions.
  • ๐Ÿ› 'Trolleyology' has faced criticism for its unrealistic premise, with some arguing that it doesn't provide meaningful insights into ethical decision-making.
  • ๐Ÿค Researchers and philosophers are working together to program ethics into machines, showing the practical implications of theoretical dilemmas.
  • ๐Ÿ”ฎ The script suggests that even hypothetical dilemmas can have real-world consequences, emphasizing the importance of ethical programming in autonomous systems.

Q & A

  • What is the trolley problem?

    -The trolley problem is an ethical dilemma devised by philosopher Philippa Foot in 1967. It presents a scenario where a runaway trolley is heading towards five workers, and you have the option to divert it onto a second track where it would hit one worker instead, forcing a decision between sacrificing one life to save five.

  • What philosophical principle does the trolley problem often invoke?

    -The trolley problem often invokes the principle of utilitarianism, which argues for the morally correct decision being the one that maximizes well-being for the greatest number of people.

  • According to the script, what percentage of respondents in one survey agreed with flipping the switch to save five workers at the cost of one?

    -In one survey mentioned in the script, about 90% of respondents agreed that it's okay to flip the switch, letting one worker die to save five.

  • How does the script describe the variation of the trolley problem involving a man on a bridge?

    -In the bridge variation of the trolley problem, you are on a bridge with a large man next to you, and the only way to stop the trolley is to push the man onto the tracks, sacrificing his life to save the five workers below.

  • What percentage of people, according to the script, believe it's okay to push the man onto the tracks in the bridge variation?

    -In the bridge variation, only about 10% of people believe it's okay to push the man onto the tracks, according to the script.

  • Why do some people find it difficult to accept the utilitarian view in the bridge variation of the trolley problem?

    -People find it difficult to accept the utilitarian view in the bridge variation because deliberately causing someone's death feels more personal and wrong, even though it's still the logical choice from a utilitarian perspective.

  • What does the script suggest about the factors influencing our moral judgment in the trolley problem?

    -The script suggests that factors such as gender, mood (e.g., watching a comedy clip), and the type of action (e.g., pushing vs. flipping a switch) can influence our moral judgment in the trolley problem.

  • What does the script indicate about the brain's response to the classic and bridge versions of the trolley problem?

    -The script indicates that both scenarios activate areas of the brain involved in conscious decision-making and emotional responses, but the emotional response and internal conflict are much stronger in the bridge version.

  • What criticisms does the script mention about the trolley problem?

    -The script mentions that some philosophers and psychologists criticize the trolley problem for being unrealistic, arguing that its premise may not be taken seriously by study participants.

  • How does the script relate the trolley problem to modern ethical challenges, such as autonomous vehicles?

    -The script relates the trolley problem to modern ethical challenges by noting that autonomous vehicles and military drones may have to make similar life-or-death decisions, emphasizing the importance of ethical programming in these systems.

  • What does the script suggest about the importance of studying hypothetical dilemmas like the trolley problem?

    -The script suggests that studying hypothetical dilemmas like the trolley problem is important because they can help us understand and program ethics into machines, which is crucial for dealing with real-world ethical challenges.

Outlines

00:00

๐Ÿš‚ The Trolley Problem: Ethical Dilemma

The Trolley Problem is introduced as a classic ethical dilemma, devised by philosopher Philippa Foot in 1967. It presents a scenario where a trolley is speeding towards five workers, and the observer has the option to divert it onto a track with one worker. This dilemma forces us to weigh the moral code against the best outcome, which in utilitarian terms, is to save the greater number of lives. The dilemma is further complicated by variations that involve pushing a man off a bridge to stop the trolley, which reveals a deeper psychological and ethical conflict between allowing death as collateral and actively causing it. The paragraph explores the philosophical principle of utilitarianism and the emotional responses it elicits, as well as the implications of such moral decisions in modern autonomous systems like driver-less cars and military drones.

Mindmap

Keywords

๐Ÿ’กTrolley Problem

The Trolley Problem is a thought experiment in ethics, devised by philosopher Philippa Foot in 1967, which presents a moral dilemma where a person must decide between sacrificing one life to save five. It is central to the video's theme of ethical decision-making. The script uses this problem to illustrate the conflict between utilitarianism and deontological ethics.

๐Ÿ’กEthical Dilemma

An ethical dilemma refers to a situation where a difficult choice must be made between two or more alternatives, both of which may involve making a moral sacrifice. The video uses the Trolley Problem as an example of such a dilemma, emphasizing the complexity of moral decision-making.

๐Ÿ’กUtilitarianism

Utilitarianism is an ethical theory that suggests the morally right action is the one that maximizes overall well-being for the greatest number of people. The script explains that in the Trolley Problem, the utilitarian approach would be to sacrifice one person to save five, as it results in the greatest good for the greatest number.

๐Ÿ’กMoral Code

A moral code refers to a set of principles or rules that guide behavior and decision-making based on ethics. The video discusses the conflict between following a moral code that prohibits causing someone's death and choosing the action with the best outcome, as per utilitarianism.

๐Ÿ’กSurvey

A survey is a method of data collection that involves asking a group of people a set of questions to gather information. The script mentions a survey that found about 90% of respondents agreed with the utilitarian choice in the Trolley Problem, indicating a general preference for the greater good.

๐Ÿ’กVirtual Reality Simulation

A virtual reality simulation is a computer-generated environment that simulates a real or imagined scenario. The script refers to studies using virtual reality to explore the Trolley Problem, finding similar results to traditional surveys, thus validating the consistency of moral judgments in different contexts.

๐Ÿ’กInnate Instincts

Innate instincts are natural, inherent tendencies or behaviors that are not learned but are genetically determined. The video suggests that people's instincts may lead them to avoid deliberately causing someone's death, even if it conflicts with utilitarian logic.

๐Ÿ’กEthics and Psychology

Ethics and psychology intersect in the study of moral judgment and behavior, exploring how psychological factors influence ethical decisions. The script highlights this intersection in the context of the Trolley Problem, noting how various factors, such as gender or mood, can influence moral judgments.

๐Ÿ’กAutonomous Systems

Autonomous systems are those that operate without human intervention. The video discusses the ethical implications of programming autonomous systems, such as driver-less cars or military drones, to make decisions that involve moral judgments and the valuation of human life.

๐Ÿ’กBrain Activity

Brain activity refers to the functioning of the brain, including the processes and responses to stimuli. The script mentions research studying brain activity in relation to the Trolley Problem, indicating that different scenarios activate distinct areas of the brain associated with decision-making and emotional responses.

๐Ÿ’กTrolleyology

Trolleyology is a term coined to describe the academic study of the Trolley Problem and its variations. The script notes criticism of Trolleyology for its unrealistic premise but also acknowledges its relevance in the context of emerging technologies and the need for ethical programming in machines.

Highlights

The trolley problem is an ethical dilemma devised by philosopher Philippa Foot in 1967.

It challenges us to decide between sacrificing one person to save five when no good choices are available.

90% of respondents in a survey agreed to flip the switch, reflecting utilitarianism's principle of maximizing well-being for the greatest number.

Utilitarianism argues that the morally correct decision is the one that maximizes happiness for the most people, even at the cost of one life.

A modified version of the trolley problem, where pushing a man to stop the trolley, reveals a different response, with only 10% agreeing to the action.

People's instincts suggest a difference between causing death directly and allowing it as a side effect.

Ethics and psychology intersect in the trolley problem, showing that moral judgments depend on more than logical pros and cons.

Gender and mood influences, such as watching a comedy clip, can affect responses to the trolley problem.

Brain activity studies show that the emotional response and internal conflict processing are stronger in the bridge version of the dilemma.

Pushing someone to their death feels more personal, evoking an emotional aversion to killing, despite it being the logical choice.

Critics argue that 'Trolleyology' lacks real-world applicability due to its unrealistic premise.

Ethical analysis is becoming increasingly important with the advent of autonomous technologies like driver-less cars.

Governments are researching autonomous military drones that may need to make decisions involving civilian casualties.

To program ethics into machines, researchers and philosophers are collaborating on the complex problem of valuing human life and judging the greater good.

Hypothetical dilemmas like the trolley problem have real-world implications, especially with the development of autonomous systems.

Transcripts

play00:06

Imagine you're watching a runaway trolley barreling down the tracks

play00:11

straight towards five workers who can't escape.

play00:15

You happen to be standing next to a switch

play00:18

that will divert the trolley onto a second track.

play00:21

Here's the problem.

play00:22

That track has a worker on it, too, but just one.

play00:28

What do you do?

play00:29

Do you sacrifice one person to save five?

play00:32

This is the trolley problem,

play00:35

a version of an ethical dilemma that philosopher Philippa Foot devised in 1967.

play00:42

It's popular because it forces us to think about how to choose

play00:45

when there are no good choices.

play00:48

Do we pick the action with the best outcome

play00:50

or stick to a moral code that prohibits causing someone's death?

play00:55

In one survey, about 90% of respondents said that it's okay to flip the switch,

play01:00

letting one worker die to save five,

play01:04

and other studies, including a virtual reality simulation of the dilemma,

play01:08

have found similar results.

play01:11

These judgments are consistent with the philosophical principle of utilitarianism

play01:16

which argues that the morally correct decision

play01:18

is the one that maximizes well-being for the greatest number of people.

play01:23

The five lives outweigh one,

play01:25

even if achieving that outcome requires condemning someone to death.

play01:30

But people don't always take the utilitarian view,

play01:33

which we can see by changing the trolley problem a bit.

play01:37

This time, you're standing on a bridge over the track

play01:40

as the runaway trolley approaches.

play01:43

Now there's no second track,

play01:44

but there is a very large man on the bridge next to you.

play01:48

If you push him over, his body will stop the trolley,

play01:52

saving the five workers,

play01:54

but he'll die.

play01:56

To utilitarians, the decision is exactly the same,

play01:59

lose one life to save five.

play02:01

But in this case, only about 10% of people

play02:04

say that it's OK to throw the man onto the tracks.

play02:08

Our instincts tell us that deliberately causing someone's death

play02:11

is different than allowing them to die as collateral damage.

play02:16

It just feels wrong for reasons that are hard to explain.

play02:20

This intersection between ethics and psychology

play02:23

is what's so interesting about the trolley problem.

play02:26

The dilemma in its many variations reveal that what we think is right or wrong

play02:30

depends on factors other than a logical weighing of the pros and cons.

play02:36

For example, men are more likely than women

play02:38

to say it's okay to push the man over the bridge.

play02:42

So are people who watch a comedy clip before doing the thought experiment.

play02:46

And in one virtual reality study,

play02:49

people were more willing to sacrifice men than women.

play02:52

Researchers have studied the brain activity

play02:55

of people thinking through the classic and bridge versions.

play02:59

Both scenarios activate areas of the brain involved in conscious decision-making

play03:04

and emotional responses.

play03:06

But in the bridge version, the emotional response is much stronger.

play03:10

So is activity in an area of the brain

play03:13

associated with processing internal conflict.

play03:16

Why the difference?

play03:18

One explanation is that pushing someone to their death feels more personal,

play03:22

activating an emotional aversion to killing another person,

play03:26

but we feel conflicted because we know it's still the logical choice.

play03:31

"Trolleyology" has been criticized by some philosophers and psychologists.

play03:36

They argue that it doesn't reveal anything because its premise is so unrealistic

play03:41

that study participants don't take it seriously.

play03:45

But new technology is making this kind of ethical analysis

play03:48

more important than ever.

play03:50

For example, driver-less cars may have to handle choices

play03:54

like causing a small accident to prevent a larger one.

play03:58

Meanwhile, governments are researching autonomous military drones

play04:01

that could wind up making decisions of whether they'll risk civilian casualties

play04:05

to attack a high-value target.

play04:09

If we want these actions to be ethical,

play04:11

we have to decide in advance how to value human life

play04:15

and judge the greater good.

play04:17

So researchers who study autonomous systems

play04:20

are collaborating with philosophers

play04:22

to address the complex problem of programming ethics into machines,

play04:27

which goes to show that even hypothetical dilemmas

play04:30

can wind up on a collision course with the real world.

Rate This
โ˜…
โ˜…
โ˜…
โ˜…
โ˜…

5.0 / 5 (0 votes)

Related Tags
Ethical DilemmaUtilitarianismMoral ChoiceTrolley ProblemDecision MakingPhilosophyPsychologyEmotionAutonomous SystemsEthics in Tech